10/16/2008
1
HOW CHILDREN HOW CHILDREN HOW CHILDREN HOW CHILDREN EXPERIENCE ABUSEEXPERIENCE ABUSE
Dynamics of VictimizationDynamics of Victimization
DYNAMICS OFDYNAMICS OF CHILD SEXUAL ABUSECHILD SEXUAL ABUSE
Help us understand the Help us understand the child’s perspective and child’s perspective and child s perspective and child s perspective and
coping strategiescoping strategies
Adapted from © CornerHouse 2002
Child Sexual Abuse Child Sexual Abuse Accommodation SyndromeAccommodation Syndrome
Roland Summit, MDRoland Summit, MD
S S -- SecrecySecrecy HH H lpl ssn ssH lpl ssn ssH H -- HelplessnessHelplessness
E E -- Entrapment and AccommodationEntrapment and Accommodation
D D -- Delayed or Unconvincing DisclosureDelayed or Unconvincing Disclosure
R R -- RetractionRetraction © Finding Words New Jersey
10/16/2008
2
SecrecySecrecy Summit, 1983Summit, 1983
Terrifying reality of child Terrifying reality of child sexual abuse: sexual abuse:
Child is alone with the offenderChild is alone with the offender Threats/promises to keep secretThreats/promises to keep secret Sh m nd iltSh m nd iltShame and guiltShame and guilt
Nondisclosure RatesNondisclosure Rates (Lyon, 2002)(Lyon, 2002) Women: 33Women: 33--92%92% Men: 42Men: 42--85% 85%
86% of sexual assaults to 86% of sexual assaults to adolescents unreportedadolescents unreported (NIJ, 2003)(NIJ, 2003) 70% of sample told to keep 70% of sample told to keep abuse secretabuse secret (Berliner & Conte, 1990)(Berliner & Conte, 1990) © Finding Words New Jersey
HelplessnessHelplessness Summit, 1983Summit, 1983
Children are subordinate to older Children are subordinate to older people, especially those in people, especially those in authority authority Perpetrators groom children in Perpetrators groom children in nonnon--sexual ways firstsexual ways firstyy Typical response of small Typical response of small creatures to terror is to freezecreatures to terror is to freeze Child’s guilt about not protesting Child’s guilt about not protesting leads to more silenceleads to more silence Victims report saying “no” would Victims report saying “no” would have only resulted in continued have only resulted in continued abuse and perhaps further harm abuse and perhaps further harm (Berliner & Conte, 1990)(Berliner & Conte, 1990)
© Finding Words New Jersey
Entrapment & AccommodationEntrapment & Accommodation Summit, 1983Summit, 1983
Typically occurs repeatedly; child Typically occurs repeatedly; child must survivemust survive Child must accommodate to Child must accommodate to increasing sexual demands increasing sexual demands increasing sexual demands increasing sexual demands Child must accommodate to Child must accommodate to increasing awareness of betrayal by increasing awareness of betrayal by caretakerscaretakers--problems in relationship problems in relationship with nonwith non--offending parentoffending parent Feel responsible for the wellFeel responsible for the well--being being of the entire familyof the entire family Special problems of male victimsSpecial problems of male victims
© Finding Words New Jersey
10/16/2008
3
Entrapment & AccommodationEntrapment & Accommodation (cont’d…)(cont’d…)
Summit, 1983Summit, 1983 Behavioral Strategies: sexualized Behavioral Strategies: sexualized behavior, lying, difficulty concentrating, behavior, lying, difficulty concentrating, substance abuse, running awaysubstance abuse, running away Psychological Strategies:Psychological Strategies:y g gy g g
FragmentingFragmenting DissociationDissociation Dissociative Identity DisorderDissociative Identity Disorder
Delayed or Unconvincing Delayed or Unconvincing DisclosureDisclosure
Summit, 1983Summit, 1983
“Most on“Most on--going sexual abuse is going sexual abuse is nevernever disclosed outside the family.”disclosed outside the family.” Disclosure in bits and piecesDisclosure in bits and piecespp Disclosed almost incidentally in state Disclosed almost incidentally in state of turmoilof turmoil Disclosed without evident feelingDisclosed without evident feeling In a sample of sexually abused In a sample of sexually abused children (N=248), 75% did not children (N=248), 75% did not disclose within a year of onset (Elliot disclose within a year of onset (Elliot & Briere, 1994).& Briere, 1994).
© Finding Words New Jersey
Delayed or Unconvincing Delayed or Unconvincing DisclosureDisclosure
Triggers for disclosure:Triggers for disclosure: Family conflictFamily conflict Incidental discovery by 3Incidental discovery by 3rdrd partypartyIncidental discovery by 3Incidental discovery by 3 partyparty Outreach educationOutreach education Protection of younger childrenProtection of younger children Medical problemsMedical problems Close friendshipClose friendship
10/16/2008
4
RetractionRetraction
Chaotic Aftermath of DisclosureChaotic Aftermath of Disclosure Family stress and disbeliefFamily stress and disbelief Potential family disintegration and guiltPotential family disintegration and guilt After disclosure child discovers the threats After disclosure child discovers the threats After disclosure, child discovers the threats After disclosure, child discovers the threats underlying the secrecy are trueunderlying the secrecy are true Unsupportive interactions with larger Unsupportive interactions with larger systems.systems.
RetractionRetraction Summit, 1983Summit, 1983
Recantation rates across studies range from Recantation rates across studies range from 44--50% 50% (Lyon, 2002)(Lyon, 2002) Recantation rates from 4Recantation rates from 4--27% 27% (London, Bruck, (London, Bruck, Ceci & Shuman, 2005)Ceci & Shuman, 2005)
Recantations most likely to occur in Recantations most likely to occur in intrafamilial abuse cases where significant intrafamilial abuse cases where significant family members are disbelieving, nonfamily members are disbelieving, non-- supportive, blaming, and stressed.supportive, blaming, and stressed.
© Finding Words New Jersey
The Traumagenic ModelThe Traumagenic Model Finkelhor & Browne, 1985Finkelhor & Browne, 1985
1.1. BetrayalBetrayal
2.2. Traumatic Traumatic SexualizationSexualization
3.3. PowerlessnessPowerlessness
4.4. StigmatizationStigmatization
Adapted from © CornerHouse 2002
10/16/2008
5
BetrayalBetrayal Finkelhor & Browne, 1985Finkelhor & Browne, 1985
Perpetrator in position of trust/authorityPerpetrator in position of trust/authority Child discovers that someone he/she Child discovers that someone he/she depended upon caused harmdepended upon caused harmp pp p Grief reactions and depression over the Grief reactions and depression over the loss of a trusted figureloss of a trusted figure Higher levels of posttraumatic and Higher levels of posttraumatic and dissociative symptoms dissociative symptoms (Leahy, Pretty, & (Leahy, Pretty, & Tenenbaum, 2004)Tenenbaum, 2004) Can lead to avoidance of close Can lead to avoidance of close relationships or to indiscriminate choicesrelationships or to indiscriminate choices
© Finding Words New Jersey
Traumatic SexualizationTraumatic Sexualization Finkelhor & Browne, 1985Finkelhor & Browne, 1985
Inappropriate conditioning of child’s Inappropriate conditioning of child’s sexual responsivenesssexual responsiveness Problematic beliefs and feelings Problematic beliefs and feelings associated with sexuality as well as associated with sexuality as well as dysfunctional arousal patterns can dysfunctional arousal patterns can developdevelop
© Finding Words New Jersey
Traumatic Sexualization (cont’d..)Traumatic Sexualization (cont’d..) Finkelhor & Browne, 1985Finkelhor & Browne, 1985
Examples:Examples: Sexual preoccupations and repetitive sexual behavior Sexual preoccupations and repetitive sexual behavior of young child victims, such as masturbation or of young child victims, such as masturbation or compulsive sex play.compulsive sex play. Display of sexual knowledge and interests that are Display of sexual knowledge and interests that are p y gp y g inappropriate to their age.inappropriate to their age. Confusion of sexuality with nurturance or power.Confusion of sexuality with nurturance or power. Promiscuity in adolescence, aversion to sex, Promiscuity in adolescence, aversion to sex, flashbacks to the molestation experience, difficulty flashbacks to the molestation experience, difficulty with arousal and orgasm, with arousal and orgasm, vaginismusvaginismus, negative , negative attitudes toward their sexuality and their bodies.attitudes toward their sexuality and their bodies.
10/16/2008
6
PowerlessnessPowerlessness Finkehlor & Browne, 1985Finkehlor & Browne, 1985
Child cannot control noxious eventsChild cannot control noxious events Child’s attempts to halt the abuse are Child’s attempts to halt the abuse are frustrated; e.g., when they unsuccessfully frustrated; e.g., when they unsuccessfully attempt to report abuseattempt to report abusep pp p Force & threat are not necessary to instill Force & threat are not necessary to instill powerlessnesspowerlessness Narratives of victims analyzed for themes of Narratives of victims analyzed for themes of perpetrator methods perpetrator methods (Leahy, Pretty, & Tenenbaum, 2004)(Leahy, Pretty, & Tenenbaum, 2004)
Created powerless victim (deception and force)Created powerless victim (deception and force) Created omnipotent perpetrator (reality Created omnipotent perpetrator (reality definition/control & manipulation)definition/control & manipulation)
© Finding Words New Jersey
Powerlessness Powerlessness (cont’d…)(cont’d…)
Finkelhor & Browne, 1985Finkelhor & Browne, 1985
Fear and anxiety symptoms: nightmares, Fear and anxiety symptoms: nightmares, phobias, hypervigilance, clinging behavior, and phobias, hypervigilance, clinging behavior, and
l l p yp g g gp yp g g g somatic complaintssomatic complaints Sense of impotence: despair, depression, Sense of impotence: despair, depression, suicidal behavior, learning problems, inability to suicidal behavior, learning problems, inability to cope with environments that continues on into cope with environments that continues on into adolescence and adulthood, revictimizationadolescence and adulthood, revictimization Compensating for powerlessness: needs to Compensating for powerlessness: needs to control and dominatecontrol and dominate
StigmatizationStigmatization Finkelhor & Browne, 1985Finkelhor & Browne, 1985
Child’s knows that the activity is deviant and taboo and Child’s knows that the activity is deviant and taboo and abuser may convey negative statementsabuser may convey negative statements Negative connotations (badness, shame, & guilt) are Negative connotations (badness, shame, & guilt) are communicated to the child, especially after disclosurecommunicated to the child, especially after disclosure-- b i t d i t th hild’ lfb i t d i t th hild’ lf i C lt l i C lt l become incorporated into the child’s selfbecome incorporated into the child’s self--image. Cultural image. Cultural meaning plays a big role.meaning plays a big role. Risk of revictimization Risk of revictimization (Classen, Palesh & Aggarwal, 2005)(Classen, Palesh & Aggarwal, 2005)
2/3 revictimization rate2/3 revictimization rate Predictors: childhood sexual abuse & severityPredictors: childhood sexual abuse & severity Associated with multiple difficulties including selfAssociated with multiple difficulties including self--blame and blame and shameshame
© Finding Words New Jersey
10/16/2008
7
How Children Experience How Children Experience AbuseAbuse
Interview ExamplesInterview ExamplesInterview ExamplesInterview Examples
Victim Responses to AbuseVictim Responses to Abuse Twaite & RodriguezTwaite & Rodriguez--Srednicki, 2004Srednicki, 2004
Whiffen & MacIntosh, 2005Whiffen & MacIntosh, 2005 KendallKendall--Tackett, Meyer & Finkelhor, 1993Tackett, Meyer & Finkelhor, 1993
Swanstone, Tebbutt, O’Toole & Oates, 1997Swanstone, Tebbutt, O’Toole & Oates, 1997
DepressionDepression Anxiety DisordersAnxiety Disorders Di i ti /di i ti di dDi i ti /di i ti di dDissociation/dissociative disordersDissociation/dissociative disorders Post traumatic stress symptomsPost traumatic stress symptoms Attachment issuesAttachment issues Fears and somatic complaintsFears and somatic complaints Sexual behavior problemsSexual behavior problems Generalized behavior problemsGeneralized behavior problems SelfSelf--esteem issuesesteem issues
Victim Responses to AbuseVictim Responses to Abuse KendallKendall--Tackett, Meyer & Finkelhor, 1993Tackett, Meyer & Finkelhor, 1993
Ketring & Feinauer, 1999Ketring & Feinauer, 1999
Victim Impact ContinuumVictim Impact Continuum
DurationDuration FrequencyFrequency ForceForce Invasiveness of abuseInvasiveness of abuse
10/16/2008
8
Victim Responses to AbuseVictim Responses to Abuse Jonzon & Lindblad, 2005 Jonzon & Lindblad, 2005
KendallKendall--Tackett, Meyer & Finkelhor, 1993Tackett, Meyer & Finkelhor, 1993
Victim Impact ContinuumVictim Impact Continuum
Other maltreatment (physical abuse)Other maltreatment (physical abuse)p yp y SupportSupport
Support from partners and friends in adulthoodSupport from partners and friends in adulthood Maternal support in childhoodMaternal support in childhood
Coping/outlookCoping/outlook
Victim Responses to AbuseVictim Responses to Abuse KKendallendall--Tackett, Meyer & Finkelhor, 1993Tackett, Meyer & Finkelhor, 1993
Victim Impact ContinuumVictim Impact Continuum
?? AgeAge •• Age at time of assessment Age at time of assessment appearsappears relatedrelatedAge at time of assessment Age at time of assessment appearsappears relatedrelated •• Difficult to assess due to intervening variablesDifficult to assess due to intervening variables
?? GenderGender •• Studies inconsistent; recommended for further Studies inconsistent; recommended for further
researchresearch
Victim Responses to AbuseVictim Responses to Abuse KendallKendall--Tackett, Meyer & Finkelhor, 1993Tackett, Meyer & Finkelhor, 1993
Ketring & Feinauer, 1999Ketring & Feinauer, 1999 Leahy, Pretty & Tenenbaum, 2004Leahy, Pretty & Tenenbaum, 2004
Victim Impact ContinuumVictim Impact Continuum
Vi timVi tim P p t t R l ti nship P p t t R l ti nship VictimVictim--Perpetrator Relationship Perpetrator Relationship Range: Father figure..other Range: Father figure..other family/acquaintance..strangerfamily/acquaintance..stranger “Closeness” of relationship“Closeness” of relationship High levels of authority/trust associated with High levels of authority/trust associated with emotional manipulation and increased symptomsemotional manipulation and increased symptoms
10/16/2008
9
Victim Responses to AbuseVictim Responses to Abuse
VictimVictim--Perpetrator Perpetrator RelationshipRelationship
TargetTarget Sees self as victimSees self as victim “Why me?”“Why me?” May be more frightened May be more frightened but less guilt riddentbut less guilt riddent
Victim Responses to AbuseVictim Responses to Abuse
VictimVictim--Perpetrator RelationshipPerpetrator Relationship
FriendFriend Begins as one of companionshipBegins as one of companionship May fill a parental gapMay fill a parental gap Possibility of protectiveness by victim Possibility of protectiveness by victim toward perpetratortoward perpetrator
Victim Responses to AbuseVictim Responses to Abuse
VictimVictim--Perpetrator Perpetrator RelationshipRelationship
SibliSibliSiblingSibling Enmeshed system in Enmeshed system in familyfamily Age or power difference Age or power difference may not be perceived by may not be perceived by victimvictim
10/16/2008
10
Victim Responses to AbuseVictim Responses to Abuse
VictimVictim--Perpetrator RelationshipPerpetrator Relationship
CaretakerCaretaker Vigilant of perpetrator’s needsVigilant of perpetrator’s needs Protective of perpetratorProtective of perpetrator May also be concerned about impact on May also be concerned about impact on rest of familyrest of family
Victim Responses to AbuseVictim Responses to Abuse VictimVictim--Perpetrator Perpetrator
RelationshipRelationship
LoverLover A d A d Age and power Age and power difference not difference not perceived by victimperceived by victim Feelings of specialnessFeelings of specialness Protective of Protective of perpetratorperpetrator
“What it was “What it was like to be an like to be an
incest incest victim”victim”
10/16/2008
11
Some day maybe there will exist a well informed, well considered and yet fervent public conviction that theconviction, that the most deadly of all possible sins is the mutilation of a child’s spirit.
- Erik Erickson © CornerHouse 2002
|
Title ABC/123 Version X |
1 |
|
Green Pastures Static Budget Income Statement ACC/561 Version 7 |
1 |
University of Phoenix Material
Green Pastures
Static Budget Income Statement
For the Year Ended December 31, 2017
Actual Master Budget Difference
Number of Mares 52 60 8 U
Number of Boarding Days 19,000 21,900 2,900 U
Sales $380,000 $547,500 $167,500 U
Less: Variable Expenses
Feed 104,390 109,500 5,110 F
Veterinary Fees 58,838 65,700 6,862 F
Blacksmith Fees 4,984 5,475 491 F
Supplies 10,178 12,045 1,867 F
Total Variable Expenses 178,390 192,720 14,330 F
Contribution Margin 201,610 354,780 153,170 U
Less: Fixed Expenses
Depreciation 40,000 40,000 -0-
Insurance 11,000 11,000 -0-
Utilities 12,000 14,000 2,000 F
Repairs and Maintenance 10,000 11,000 1,000 F
Labor 88,000 95,000 7,000 F
Advertisement 12,000 8,000 4,000 U
Entertainment 7,000 5,000 2,000 U
Total Fixed Expenses 180,000 184,000 4,000 F
Net Income $21,610 $170,780 $149,170 U
Copyright © XXXX by University of Phoenix. All rights reserved.
Copyright © 2017 by University of Phoenix. All rights reserved.
27W i n t e r 2 0 0 6 • J u v e n i l e a n d F a m i l y C o u r t J o u r n a l
II N T R O D U C T I O N Cases involving child sexual
abuse (CSA) are among the
most heartbreaking cases a
judge hears, for both emo-
tional and legal reasons.
How does the court evalu-
ate the testimony of the
young child who says it
never happened, the child
who discloses months
after the alleged event,
the girl who recants after
visitation with family mem-
bers, or the boy with an IQ of 51 who cannot
clearly articulate how the alleged abuse occurred?
Proving child sexual abuse in the absence of physi-
cal evidence or testimony of an eyewitness is difficult.
Children recant, child development issues intervene,
and cognitive limitations raise questions, while the testi-
mony of the adult perpetrator does not waiver. Indeed,
although errors in either direction can have devastating
consequences, finding the truth in CSA cases seems too
often impossible. Decision makers confront the twin
specters of leaving inarticulate children unprotected
from further traumatic sexual abuse on the one hand,
and subjecting innocent caregivers to criminal prosecu-
tion or the loss of parental
rights on the other.
Science can be a tre-
mendous asset to judges in
understanding and interpret-
ing the behavior of victims
of child sexual abuse, since
their behavior can often
seem counterintuitive. It is
essential that judges consider
children’s disclosure patterns
in light of current research in
order to have the greatest
chance to evaluate the facts
and find the truth. Children’s disclosure patterns are
crucial because physical findings are diagnostic of child
sexual abuse in 10% or fewer cases (Frasier & Makaroff,
this issue). Sexual abuse, especially when there is no
penetration, rarely results in physical trauma. Even when
there has been sexual penetration, the capacity for rapid
healing of the genital anatomy inhibits the detection of
evidence (The National Research Council, 1993, p. 72).
Therefore, children’s statements are central both to the
prosecution of the crime of child sexual abuse and the
protection of children from further abuse.
This review is intended to update criminal, juve-
nile, and domestic relations court judges who preside
The State of the Debate About Children’s Disclosure Patterns
in Child Sexual Abuse Cases BY ERNA OLAFSON AND JUDGE CINDY S. LEDERMAN
A B S T R A C T In current research studies about the disclosure patterns of sexually abused children, experts agree that most victims delay disclosure for years, often until adulthood. Researchers disagree about dis- closure rates and recantation rates among children during formal interviews. Studies of children who had not previously disclosed but are known through corroborative evidence to have been sexually abused show lower rates of disclosure than do studies of children who had disclosed prior to the formal interview. Gradual disclosures among children are common, and more than a single interview may be necessary in some cases. Prior disclosure, level of support by non-offending parents, developmental level, and relationship to perpetrator affect children’s rates of disclosure and their disclosure patterns. More research is necessary to clarify children’s post-disclo- sure recantation rates and predictors.
Erna Olafson, Ph.D., Psy.D., is Associate Professor of Clinical Psychiatry and Pediatrics at Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center and the University of Cincinnati College of Medicine. She is Director of the Program on Child Abuse Forensic and Treatment Training at Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Childhood Trust. Her presentations and publications focus on domestic violence, sexual assault, and child abuse. Judge Cindy S. Lederman is the Presiding Judge of the Juvenile Court in Miami-Dade County, Florida. A member of the NCJFCJ’s Board of Trustees, she was instrumental in creating the Dade County Domestic Violence Court and served as the court’s first Administrative Judge. In 1999, Judge Lederman was awarded a Fellowship from Zero to Three: The National Center for Infants, Toddlers and Families in their Leaders of the 21st Century Initiative.
C h i l d r e n ' s D i s c l o s u r e P a t t e r n s i n S e x u a l A b u s e C a s e s
28 J u v e n i l e a n d F a m i l y C o u r t J o u r n a l • W i n t e r 2 0 0 6
over CSA cases about current areas of agreement and
disagreement among scientific researchers about the
disclosure patterns of CSA victims. A major volume on
abuse disclosure patterns is scheduled for publication
in 2006 (Pipe, Lamb, Orbach, & Cederborg, in press).
It contains chapters by researchers from differing
perspectives that we have drawn upon for this article
(London, Bruck, Ceci, & Shuman, in press; Lyon, in press).
Unfortunately for those charged with making decisions
about children’s welfare, no single school of researchers
has the last word on these controversial issues.
Brief History Many scholarly papers about children’s disclosure
patterns either begin with a discussion of Roland
Summit’s Child Sexual Abuse Accommodation Syndrome
(CSAAS) or structure their arguments around his model
of children’s behavior in these cases (London, Bruck,
Ceci, & Shuman, 2005; Lyon, 2002; Lyon, in press;
Summit, 1983). Summit argued that children often deny
being sexually abused, even when they are directly
asked, and that disclosing children often subsequently
recant their allegations. He based the accommodation
syndrome primarily on cases of intrafamilial child
sexual abuse (incest) rather than extrafamilial child
sexual abuse.
Although Summit’s “syndrome” has been litigated
with a variety of outcomes in many courts, it may have
become so controversial that it obscures rather than
clarifies the issues at hand. Judges should bear in mind
that for almost a century before Summit published his
influential paper, there was statistical evidence that
children often delay disclosure or remain completely
silent about sexual victimization. Indeed, this prior
literature was so extensive that a major psychological
journal rejected Summit’s accommodation syndrome
paper before it found publication elsewhere because,
the reviewers argued, it contributed nothing new
(Lyon, in press; Olafson, 2002). There have also been a
number of studies documenting children’s disclosure
patterns in otherwise corroborated child sexual abuse
cases since the 1983 publication of Summit’s paper
(Lyon, in press). Examining children’s disclosure pat-
terns one category at a time, without organizing them
around Summit’s now-controversial accommodation
syndrome, may clarify and simplify the issues.
The Issues What are the disclosure and non-disclosure patterns
among children known to have been sexually abused?
There are several issues:
■ Do most child victims delay reporting sexual abuse, sometimes until adulthood?
■ If directly asked, do most child victims disclose sexual abuse?
■ If directly asked, do some CSA victims initially fail to disclose or deny being abused, so that more than one formal interview becomes necessary?
■ How common is incremental abuse disclosure, from partial and fragmentary accounts to full disclosure over time?
■ Once children have disclosed sexual abuse, do a high percentage of known victims subsequently recant or retract their disclosures?
■ Are there factors such as gender, developmental level, culture, degree of abuse severity, parental sup- port, and relationship to perpetrator that influence disclosure patterns among CSA victims?
Sources of Information
The two most reliable sources of information about
disclosure patterns in CSA victims are:
■ Retrospective surveys of adults who report having been sexually abused during childhood; and
■ Research about children’s statements during evaluation and treatment in cases with cor- roborative evidence that is independent of children’s statements, such as videotapes of the actual abuse, physical findings, sexually trans- mitted diseases, and offender confession.
Both sources are imperfect. Cases that have indepen-
dent corroboration may be unrepresentative of sexual
abuse cases in general. Retrospective surveys depend on
human memory over time, so that under-reporting, over-
reporting, and inaccurate reporting may occur.
Nevertheless, cases with independent corrobora-
tion and retrospective surveys are superior to the
other sources sometimes used in literature reviews. For
example, studies that claim substantiation or conviction
rates as “independent” corroboration may significantly
inflate the percentages of actually abused children who
disclose their victimization during formal question-
ing. This is because substantiation, prosecution, and
E r n a O l a f s o n a n d J u d g e C i n d y S . L e d e r m a n
29W i n t e r 2 0 0 6 • J u v e n i l e a n d F a m i l y C o u r t J o u r n a l
conviction depend so heavily at all decision stages on
children’s statements. To argue that substantiation rates
that depend upon children’s disclosures proves that
most children make disclosures when interviewed is to
argue in a circle (Lyon, in press).
The definitions of key terms also affect research out-
comes, but researchers do not always specify their oper-
ational definitions. “Child sexual abuse” can include a
wide variety of behaviors, from non-contact exposure to
genital fondling to violent genital, oral, and anal rape. In
this article, we focus primarily on contact child sexual
abuse. “Disclosure” also has a variety of meanings. We
define disclosure to mean a clear verbal statement that
at least one abusive act took place, although a disclosure
need not be a complete report of everything that hap-
pened. Our definition does not include suggestive doll
play and other fragmentary “partial disclosures” that,
when included in research studies, artificially inflate
children’s “disclosure” rates (e.g. Dubowitz, Black, &
Harrington, 1992).
“Non-disclosure” can also vary in meaning depend-
ing on whether it refers to a child’s non-disclosure dur-
ing a single or initial interview or a child’s non-disclo-
sure maintained over six or more interviews. Children
questioned only once show higher “non-disclosure”
rates than do children questioned several times, so that
studies such as that by Sorenson & Snow (1991) that
show very high initial non-disclosure rates have an even-
tual disclosure rate of over 90%.
Child Sexual Abuse Disclosures Delayed Until Adulthood
There appears to be a consensus among research-
ers that most child sexual abuse victims delay disclos-
ing, often until adulthood. A number of well-designed
retrospective surveys now show that the great majority
of victims delay disclosing contact child sexual abuse
during childhood (Finkelhor, Hotaling, Lewis, & Smith,
1990; Smith et al., 2000). These surveys also indicate
that even when adults recall having told someone
about the abuse, the majority of these cases were not
then reported to the authorities. In one survey, 28% of
respondents stated that they had disclosed to no one
before telling the telephone interviewer about the child
sexual abuse (Smith et al., 2000); another survey found
that 42% of men and 33% of women first told anyone
about having been sexually abused as children when
asked during the retrospective telephone interview
(Finkelhor et al., 1990).
London and colleagues (2005) summarize the ret-
rospective literature by noting that the results of 10
retrospective surveys indicate that only one-third of
adults who suffered child sexual abuse revealed the
abuse to anyone during childhood. The study concludes
that “approximately 60%-70% of adults do not recall
ever disclosing their abuse as children, and only a small
minority of participants (10%-18%) recalled that their
cases were reported to the authorities” (London et al.,
2005, p. 203). Although London and colleagues note the
research limitations inherent in adult retrospective liter-
ature, they also write, “Given the differences in method-
ology, definitions of abuse, and sample characteristics,
the general consistency of these findings across these
studies is noteworthy” (London et al., 2005, p. 201; but
see Poole & Dickinson, 2005).
Judges and other fact finders can only adjudicate
those cases that come to their attention, and a child’s
prior disclosure to a caregiver or friend constitutes
the most common means by which child sexual abuse
comes to the attention of the authorities and thus to the
courts (Lyon, in press). Therefore, because it appears
that most people delay disclosing until adulthood, chil-
dren who decide to tell someone about being sexually
abused and whose cases therefore come to court are
not representative of sexually abused children in gen-
eral. In other words, child protection authorities and
the judiciary are likely to see only a minority of those
children who are actually being sexually abused. There
are, of course, some sexual abuse cases that are report-
ed for reasons other than a child’s prior disclosure, such
as children’s sexualized behaviors, physical findings,
and other external evidence. This review article focuses
on the disclosure patterns and behaviors among both
groups of sexually abused children, those who had pre-
viously disclosed and a smaller number of those who
came into the system in some other way.
Child Sexual Abuse Disclosures Delayed within Childhood
There appears to be agreement among researchers
from diverse perspectives that “when children do dis-
close, it often takes them a long time to do so” (London
C h i l d r e n ' s D i s c l o s u r e P a t t e r n s i n S e x u a l A b u s e C a s e s
30 J u v e n i l e a n d F a m i l y C o u r t J o u r n a l • W i n t e r 2 0 0 6
et al., 2005, p. 204). In a study of 399 children aged 8
to 15, Elliott and Briere (1994) find that of 248 subjects
assessed as having been sexually abused, 74.9% did not
disclose their abuse to anyone within the year that it first
occurred, and 17.8% had waited more than five years to
tell anyone. The courts are likely to see many such cases
in which children delayed reporting for months or even
years before telling someone about the abuse. It is also
not unusual for children to disclose the abuse long after
adjudication when they are in a safe environment and
the litigation is finished. Delays in telling anyone about
the abuse for several months, a year, or even longer
occur in a significant percentage of child sexual abuse
cases (Henry, 1997; Sas & Cunningham, 1995).
In weighing the evidence in child sexual abuse cases, judges and other fact finders should be aware that, in a high percentage of actual CSA cases, there will be delays of months or even years between the onset of the abuse and a child first disclosing to another person.
Children’s Gradual Disclosures during Formal Interviews
Many prosecutors are familiar with the problem of
incremental disclosure, in which a child may disclose
only aspects of an abusive event, such as genital fon-
dling, during the initial interview. Shortly before trial
is scheduled to begin, the child, perhaps during court
preparation with the prosecutor, describes new details,
such as penetrating oral sex, that necessitate postpone-
ments, the filing of new criminal charges, and concerns
about the child’s credibility and competence. In one
such case, a young incest victim, when asked why she
had not mentioned crucial additional information dur-
ing her initial advocacy center interviews responded,
“I just didn’t think of it.” This pattern of partial disclo-
sure can be explained by Summit’s classic child sexual
abuse accommodation syndrome, but it may also simply
reflect the usual patterns of recall in the very young.
In an experimental study, Dr. Robyn Fivush asked non-
abused children aged 3-6 about a known event on
two subsequent occasions (Fivush, 1994). On the two
recall occasions, children reported different but still
accurate information about the events, with an overlap
of details between the two retellings of only 20%. This
research about children’s normal patterns of recollec-
tion and reporting could in itself justify recommending
that children be given more than a single interview to
tell the authorities about the events in their lives.
In a summary of 21 studies from 1965 to 1993 of
children diagnosed with gonorrhea, Lyon finds gradual
disclosure by children to be very common (Lyon, in
press). In 118 CSA cases studied by Elliott and Briere
(1994), there was external evidence for the abuse,
including, for example, medical evidence diagnostic of
child sexual abuse, perpetrator confession, a witness
to the abuse, or pornographic pictures of the child.
In a number of these 118 cases, victims disclosed par-
tially in the first interview by mentioning fondling, but
when investigators confronted them with the external
evidence for more severe abuse (penetration), the chil-
dren then made more complete disclosures.
Thus, when questioned during formal interviews,
children may only partially disclose during the initial
interview. Because evidentiary studies show that trau-
matic medical evidence (such as a ruptured hymen) is
lacking in a significant number of cases in which per-
petrators have confessed to penile penetration, judges
should not prematurely regard children’s statements
as complete after a single interview (Muram, Speck, &
Gold, 1991). As Elliott and Briere (1994) write, “Forensic
evaluations that consist of a single interview may result
in incomplete disclosure and less accurate determina-
tions, especially in cases where medical or other exter-
nal data are lacking or inconclusive” (p. 274).
This recommendation does not contradict the long-
held principle in the child protection fields to avoid
subjecting children to repeated interviews by multiple
investigators from social services, law enforcement, and
the court system. The National Children’s Advocacy
Center has developed and tested guidelines for extend-
ed forensic evaluations with reticent children. If several
interviews become necessary, it is recommended that a
single interviewer conduct them and that the question-
ing be sensitively structured to build rapport over time
and avoid repetitive questioning and suggestiveness
(Carnes, Wilson, & Nelson-Gardell, 1999; Carnes, Nelson-
Gardell, Wilson, & Orgassa, 2001).
E r n a O l a f s o n a n d J u d g e C i n d y S . L e d e r m a n
31W i n t e r 2 0 0 6 • J u v e n i l e a n d F a m i l y C o u r t J o u r n a l
Because many sexually abused children in externally corroborated cases are known to disclose only gradually, more than a single interview may become necessary to serve children’s safety and justice. See the guide- lines by the National Children’s Advocacy Center (Carnes et al., 1999; 2001).
Non-Disclosure or Denial by Children When Interviewed about Child Sexual Abuse
The most troubling cases for the courts are those in
which there are red flags indicating a strong possibility of
child sexual abuse: The case is reported, the child inter-
viewed, and the child discloses no sexual abuse. There are
two classes of children to consider here:
■ Children who previously disclosed partially or fully to another person and thus precipitated entry into the system; and
■ Children who came into the system through other means, such as diagnosis of a sexually transmitted disease during routine medical care, extreme sexual- ized behaviors, or the discovery of videotapes docu- menting the abuse.
It is about children’s disclosure patterns once they
are in the system that the experts disagree, and these
cases are the most troubling to those responsible for
protecting children from abuse and protecting adults
from false allegations.
London et al. (2005) state that “the data clearly demon-
strate that most children who are interviewed about sexual
abuse do disclose and do not later recant…” (p. 217).
Lyon (in press) responds with a critique that reveals
problems with two kinds of case selection bias in many
of the samples upon which London and colleagues
based the above conclusion. Lyon argues that:
■ To avoid suspicion bias, one must examine cases that did not come to the attention of the authorities because a child disclosed to someone prior to the formal interview; and
■ To avoid substantiation bias, one must examine cases in which substantiation was completely inde- pendent of the child’s statements.
To understand how both forms of selection bias
artificially inflate the actual rates of children’s sexual
abuse disclosures, consider the following extreme case.
If we suspect sexual abuse only when a child has previ-
ously disclosed, then 100% of children in a sample of
children suspected of being sexually abused will have
disclosed at some point. If we substantiate child sexual
abuse only if a child discloses, then 100% of children
in a sample of substantiated cases will have disclosed.
The reality is only somewhat less extreme. The great
majority of suspected CSA cases come to our attention
only because a child has previously disclosed. Child
sexual abuse substantiation also depends most heavily
on children’s disclosures, because external evidence of
child sexual abuse (such as physical findings or offender
confession) is rare and generally detected only after
sexual abuse has been suspected.
London et al. (2005) seem to agree with Lyon about
suspicion bias by writing, “Prior disclosure of abuse pre-
dicts disclosure during formal assessment” (p. 209), but
they do not then systematically deal with the problem of
suspicion bias. London and colleagues also acknowledge
but do not fully address the substantiation bias problem
by writing, “In many of the cited studies, classification of
abuse was often based in part on children’s disclosures;
consequently, the conclusion that abused children do
disclose abuse during formal interviews may be circular”
(p. 217). They then base their conclusion that “the evi-
dence fails to support the notion that denials, tentative
disclosures, and recantations characterize the disclosure
patterns of children with validated histories of sexual
abuse” (p. 194) on their review of research studies that
are in many cases flawed by both suspicion and substan-
tiation bias. What do studies that avoid both biases tell
us about this area of contention?
Studies of Disclosure Patterns in Cases without Selection Bias
Nine boys and one girl were interviewed by police
after Swedish law enforcement discovered videotapes of
102 incidents of child sexual abuse, ranging from expo-
sure of the child’s genitals to oral/anal/vaginal intercourse
(Sjoberg & Lindblad, 2002). The perpetrator was either
related to the children or knew them through his work
at a day care center. Abuse severity was coded both from
the videotapes and from children’s statements. No child
had previously disclosed abuse nor had it been suspected.
Five children reported no abuse during police interviews,
for a disclosure rate of 50%. The child who had suffered
C h i l d r e n ' s D i s c l o s u r e P a t t e r n s i n S e x u a l A b u s e C a s e s
32 J u v e n i l e a n d F a m i l y C o u r t J o u r n a l • W i n t e r 2 0 0 6
the greatest number (60 incidents) and most severe
sexual assaults according to the videotaped evidence did
not disclose during the police interview. Two of the five
children who did disclose did so only in response to lead-
ing questions. No child reported any sexual behavior not
documented on the videotape.
Cases with children not suspected to be sexual abuse
victims who are diagnosed with sexually transmitted dis-
eases, who are too old to have acquired the diseases con-
genitally and too young to have acquired them through
consensual sex with peers, also avoid both suspicion
and substantiation bias. Confining this review to STD
diagnosis deals with the problem raised by London et al.
(2005) that “medical evidence” is not always a “reliable
benchmark” because, for example, genital redness may be
caused by many things besides sexual abuse.
Lawson and Chaffin (1992) found that among 28 chil-
dren in which STDs were medically diagnosed without
prior suspicion of abuse, only 12 children (43%) made
an allegation of sexual abuse during the initial formal
interview, and 16 children did not. Almost half of these
children had shown no physical or behavioral symptoms
of sexual abuse, so that there were no “red flags” that
would have otherwise brought these children into the
system as possible CSA victims. Maternal attitude influ-
enced disclosure patterns greatly. Among those children
whose parents were supportive, 63% disclosed abuse
during these initial interviews, whereas when caregivers
expressed skepticism, only 17% disclosed.
Of the 16 false negatives in the original Lawson
and Chaffin study, five were subsequently located and
consented to be interviewed. Four of these five had a
supportive parent and one a non-supportive parent.
Researchers presented the study to parents and chil-
dren as an evaluation of responses to prior emergency
room visits, and they never mentioned child abuse.
Nevertheless, four of the five parents spontaneously
told the researchers that their children had disclosed
sexual abuse some time after the initial hospital inter-
view, a finding that supports the idea that CSA disclo-
sure is often an incremental process that may require
more than a single interview (Chaffin, Lawson, Selby,
& Wherry, 1997). Upon psychological testing, the four
non-disclosing children whose parents had been sup-
portive at the time of the initial interview tested three
times higher on dissociative symptoms than did the dis-
closing children and nine times higher on dissociative
symptoms than non-abused control children. Because of
the nature of this study and the very small numbers of
children involved, these results are far from conclusive,
but they do suggest a possible link between dissociative
symptoms and non-disclosure among CSA victims.
London et al. explain the Lawson and Chaffin
results by describing this sample as “unusual” and as
representing “the small hard core of children who do
not disclose abuse when directly asked” (2005, p. 215).
Lyon argues in response that the Lawson and Chaffin
sample avoids the problems of suspicion and substan-
tiation bias that characterize many other samples. Lyon
then raises a concern about the many cases that are
closed as unsubstantiated after a single interview during
which a possibly sexually abused child without medical
evidence fails to disclose when formally questioned.
A number of other samples document similarly
low rates of disclosure in STD cases. Lyon examined 21
studies published between 1965 and 1993 of children
diagnosed with gonorrhea. In nine of these papers, the
authors referred to a “history” of sexual contact or sexual
abuse for some of the children with gonorrhea, without
clarifying whether this history came from children’s dis-
closures or from other sources (Lyon, in press). In most
of the remaining studies, the authors used words such as
“admitted” or “denied” sexual contact or referred even
more directly to children’s statements. Even when all
the cases of “history” were counted as actual child dis-
closures, Lyon finds that the average rate of “disclosure”
among the 579 children in these studies was 43%, or 250
children. Given the broad definition of “disclosure” that
he applies here, Lyon argues that this may actually be an
overestimate of disclosure rates. Most of these studies
indicated that the medical professionals questioned the
children, but the precise nature of these questions is not
known. When Lyon omits studies with children younger
than three years of age to control for developmental
limitations on narrative skill, he finds that 185 of 437
children, or 42%, disclosed.
To summarize this sample of disclosure studies that
avoid both suspicion and substantiation bias, Sjoberg
and Lindblad find a disclosure rate of 50%, Lawson and
Chaffin find a disclosure rate of 43%, and in a review of
21 studies of children diagnosed with gonorrhea, Lyon
finds a disclosure rate of 43%. London et al. (2005) assert
E r n a O l a f s o n a n d J u d g e C i n d y S . L e d e r m a n
33W i n t e r 2 0 0 6 • J u v e n i l e a n d F a m i l y C o u r t J o u r n a l
that when CSA victims are interviewed, a “majority” of
them disclose sexual abuse. These 23 studies contradict
that assertion by showing that only from 42% to 50%
of children known through external evidence to have
been sexually abused actually disclosed during their
formal interviews.
We agree with London and colleagues that “If the field
is to be guided by scientifically validated concepts then
this must be predicated on the literature that comes clos-
est to the standards of science” (2005, p. 220). Research
studies that avoid suspicion bias and substantiation bias
come closer to this scientific standard than do research
studies that suffer from one or both of these biases, and
these studies show far lower rates of children’s disclosure
of child sexual abuse than London et al. (2005) assert.
When children who have not previously disclosed are interviewed, and these chil- dren are known to have been sexually abused because of external corroborating evidence, their rates of disclosure range from 42% to 50%.
Studies of Child Sexual Abuse Cases that Avoid Only Substantiation Bias
Studies of previously disclosing children will gener-
ally show higher rates of disclosure than do studies in
which children had not previously disclosed, because
prior disclosure predicts children’s disclosure during for-
mal interviews. The majority of cases that judges are likely
to see will involve previously disclosing children, because
child sexual abuse is most often suspected when a child
says something to a caregiver or friend that brings the
case into the system. However, many research studies do
not fully document whether or not a child disclosed prior
to entering the system. Others state how many children
disclosed to another person prior to the formal interview.
Both categories are reviewed in this section.
Hershkowitz, Horowitz, & Lamb (2005) examined
all interviews with alleged victims of sexual abuse, aged
3 to 14, in Israel from 1998 to 2002 (10,988 interviews).
Most of the alleged victims were aged 7 to 14. During
one interview, 71.1% of these children made allega-
tions of child sexual abuse. Boys were less likely than
girls to allege sexual abuse. Children aged 3-6 were less
likely to make allegations than children aged 7-10, and
children aged 11-14 had the highest rates of allegation.
Children were much less likely to make allegations
when the suspect was a parent or parent-figure. This
very large study confirms patterns observed in smaller
U.S. samples. However, because of limitations in the data
set, the authors did not state which children had made
disclosures prior to the formal interviews, although it
is known that prior disclosure is the primary means
by which cases come into the system (Lyon, in press).
The authors were also unable to determine from the
data set which children had been interviewed more
than one time. Finally, it was not possible to analyze
separately those cases that had independent evidence
corroborating child sexual abuse, so that the validity
and non-validity of the children’s allegations could not
be determined.
Elliott and Briere (1994) find that 39 of 118 (33%)
children aged 8 to 15 for whom there was external evi-
dence of child sexual abuse made no disclosure about
having been sexually abused during formal interviews,
and some of the remaining 67% of children with exter-
nal evidence who did disclose required more than one
interview to do so. Twenty of these children had report-
edly disclosed to another person before the interview
but did not do so during the interview, and 19 disclosed
to no one either before or during the formal interview.
A higher percentage of the non-disclosing children had
mothers who were not supportive. There was a higher
percentage of African-American children among the
non-disclosing group. Victims were aged eight through
adolescence, and other research has shown that school-
aged children and adolescents are more likely to dis-
close sexual abuse when questioned than are young-
er children (DiPietro, Runyan, & Frederickson, 1997;
Hershkowitz et al., 2005; Keary & Fitzpatrick, 1994;
London et al., 2005; Lyon, in press; Sas & Cunningham,
1995). London et al. mistakenly calculate a disclosure
rate of 84% in the Elliott and Briere study, a percentage
that is inflated because of substantiation bias. London
and colleagues (in press) calculated the 39 non-dis-
closers against the 248 children classified as “abused,”
although the 248 substantiation figure includes over
100 children classified by the researchers as abused
because they made “consistent, detailed, contextually
embedded, developmentally age-appropriate accounts
of at least one abusive incident” (Elliott & Briere, 1994, p.
264). When substantiation bias is eliminated and the 39
C h i l d r e n ' s D i s c l o s u r e P a t t e r n s i n S e x u a l A b u s e C a s e s
34 J u v e n i l e a n d F a m i l y C o u r t J o u r n a l • W i n t e r 2 0 0 6
children who did not disclose during formal interviews
are measured against the 118 cases with corroborative
evidence independent of children’s disclosures, the
disclosure rate during formal interviews is 67% and the
non-disclosure rate of known victims is 33%.
In their forthcoming chapter, London et al. also
cite inflated 75% disclosure statistics from a study by
Dubowitz et al. (1992). There were 28 children in that
study who had medical examination findings indicative
of child sexual abuse, and of these, 13 fully disclosed, 7
did not disclose, and 8 “partially disclosed.” London et al.
(in press) must be including the 8 partial disclosers in
this high percentage, although these partial “disclosures”
are described by Dubowitz et al. (1992) as “suggestive
doll play or an inconclusive account of alleged abuse”
(p. 690). When only real disclosures are included, the
disclosure rate in the Dubowitz study is 46%.
Finally, because of methodological shortcomings
in two older studies, Sorenson and Snow (1991) and
Bradley and Wood (1996), they are reviewed only briefly
here. Sorenson and Snow report a 72% initial non-disclo-
sure rate by children, and Bradley and Wood report a 7%
total non-disclosure rate apparently over the course of
several interviews. The end results for both studies do
not differ greatly. Bradley and Wood write that 95% of
the children in cases that had external evidence of child
sexual abuse similar to that used by Sorenson and Snow
“made a partial or full disclosure of abuse during at least
one interview with DPRS or police” (p. 885). In their
evaluation and treatment sample, Sorenson and Snow
write that 96% of the children for which there was
external evidence eventually reached “active” disclosure,
often after weeks or months of treatment.
Prior disclosure predicts disclosure during formal interviews. However, in externally corroborated cases in which children have previously disclosed, a substantial percent- age of children do not disclose during the first formal interview. Many of these chil- dren do disclose if given the opportunity in subsequent interviews.
Recantations
A 10-year-old girl who has told investigators that
she was repeatedly sodomized by her soccer coach
comes to the witness stand during criminal proceed-
ings, freezes, and mumbles to the jury that she “cannot
remember” what happened; as with many cases of anal
penetration, there is no medical evidence. An adoles-
cent boy who has told his school counselor that his
stepmother “messes with my dick” explains to the child
protection investigator the next day that he was “just
kidding.” A preschool girl who has reportedly told her
divorced mother that her daddy “tickles my coochie
and it hurts,” climbs under a table during the advocacy
center interview and denies ever visiting her father. Are
these children withdrawing their allegations because
they were never abused, or are they recanting true state-
ments about abusive events?
These are among the most challenging cases to
investigate and to litigate. There are far fewer studies on
recantation than on delay, non-disclosure, and disclosure,
and there is not yet definitive research about recanta-
tion rates in externally validated cases. Recantation
rates in various studies range from 4% (Bradley & Wood,
1996) to 22% (Sorenson & Snow, 1991). Most studies
of recantation rates contain serious methodological
flaws. Therefore, we cannot agree with the statement
by London et al. (2005) that “only a small percentage of
children in these studies recant” (p. 217). It is more accu-
rate to state that we simply do not yet know how often
and why children recant their statements about actually
having been sexually abused.
There is research currently under way. Malloy, Lyon,
Quas, and Forman (2005) recently presented results
from a random sample of 217 substantiated CSA cases
from the Los Angeles Dependency Court in 1999-2000
to discern disclosure patterns across all interviews.
Children were aged 2 to 17, and 90% were female. Most
of the children had from 3 to 9 interviews. The major-
ity (78%) had disclosed to someone prior to the police
or social services interview, so that the low initial non-
disclosure rate of 9% can be explained by this sample’s
suspicion bias. Twenty-three percent of the children
fully recanted their allegations at some point, and an
additional 11% minimized the severity of the abuse they
had initially reported by partially recanting, for a total of
34% full or partial recanters. Lack of maternal support
and abuse by a male caretaker were predictors for full
recantation. In cases that had medical evidence cor-
roborating the sexual abuse, 25% of the children either
fully or partially recanted the allegation, and 24.5% of
E r n a O l a f s o n a n d J u d g e C i n d y S . L e d e r m a n
35W i n t e r 2 0 0 6 • J u v e n i l e a n d F a m i l y C o u r t J o u r n a l
children whose perpetrator confessed recanted at some
point during the evaluation. The authors conclude that
recantation is not rare in externally corroborated cases
and in substantiated cases, when all interviews in each
case are examined.
Recantations should not be interpreted to mean that
an allegation is necessarily false. Unfortunately, criminal
courts do not always agree. For example, in Florida, a
prior inconsistent statement from a recanting alleged
victim of child sexual abuse is not sufficient in and of
itself to sustain conviction, even if repeated on multiple
occasions (State v. Green 667 So.2d 756 Fla.,1995. West’s
F.S.A § 90.803(23)).
Researchers have not established whether recantations are frequent or infrequent, but they do occur in externally corrobo- rated CSA cases, especially when abuse was by a male caregiver and/or maternal support was absent.
Bizarre Disclosures
Many children’s cases never reach the courts
because they contain bizarre and impossible details.
These can include accounts of, for example, having
been abused aboard rocket ships, having been abused
by the Wizard of Oz, having been stabbed all over the
body (without medical evidence), having murdered
and dissected a baby, and other grotesque and extreme
statements. In a random sample of 104 child sexual
abuse and physical abuse “gold standard” cases with two
forms of external evidence selected from a child protec-
tion facility, the blind scoring of transcribed disclosure
statements shows that 15.38% of the most severe cases
with victims aged 4-9 contained such implausible details
(Dalenberg, 1996; Dalenberg, Hyland, & Cuevas, 2002).
These fantastic statements were from cases in which the
researchers could be certain that physical and/or sexual
abuse had actually taken place. The rate of bizarre state-
ments in the mild, externally verified cases from this
sample was less than 4%. Because both true and false
allegations can contain implausible details, their pres-
ence does not help investigators sort truth from fiction.
What this study does indicate is that implausible details
in an otherwise solid disclosure do not in themselves
prove that an allegation is false. Indeed, these fantastic
elements may indicate that the child experienced espe-
cially severe physical and/or sexual abuse.
Variables that Affect Disclosure Patterns
We agree with London and colleagues (in press) that
future research with a multivariate model is necessary to
find causal explanations for children’s disclosure patterns,
but there are some trends that seem to be emerging.
■ Maternal or parental support: Children who lack caregiver support are far less likely to disclose than are children who have a supportive caregiver, when “support” is defined as a willingness to believe that the child sexual abuse could have happened (Elliott & Briere, 1994; Lawson & Chaffin, 1992). Elliott & Carnes (2001) find that a majority of mothers either believe or support children in CSA cases. Those cases that reach the courts may differ in crucial ways. Thus, in dependency court, familial support is often absent, hence the intervention of the state in the parent- child relationship to protect the child. The victim child, and often her siblings, are removed from their home, and sometimes there is an arrest of a family member who may be the breadwinner. In too many cases, the child is blamed, feels responsible for break- ing up the family, and eventually recants (Malloy et al., 2005).
■ Relationship to perpetrator: In some cases, the child is dissuaded from disclosing the abuse by fam- ily members who do not believe the child and wish to prevent shame and embarrassment to the family. Most studies demonstrate lower rates of disclosure or longer delays in doing so when abuse is by a family member rather than by a non-family member (Goodman-Brown, Edelstein, Goodman, Jones, & Gordon, 2003; Hershkowitz et al., 2005; Sjoberg & Lindblad, 2002; Smith et al., 2000; but see also Lamb & Edgar-Smith, 1994; London et al., 2005).
■ Age: Retrospective surveys indicate that victims first abused during adolescence are more likely to dis- close than are younger children, and they are more likely to disclose first to another adolescent than to a caregiver. Retrospective surveys also indicate that school-aged children are more likely first to reveal child sexual abuse to a parent than to another child (London et al., 2005, p. 201).
■ Gender: In both retrospective surveys and child samples, there are suggestions that boys may be more reluctant to disclose than girls, although other abuse-specific variables may influence gender differ- ences (Ghetti & Goodman, 2001; Goodman-Brown et al., 2003; Hershkowitz et al., 2005; Kendall-
C h i l d r e n ' s D i s c l o s u r e P a t t e r n s i n S e x u a l A b u s e C a s e s
36 J u v e n i l e a n d F a m i l y C o u r t J o u r n a l • W i n t e r 2 0 0 6
Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993; Levesque, 1994; London et al., 2005; Sas & Cunningham, 1995; Sauzier, 1989; Widom & Morris, 1997).
■ Culture: Although more research needs to be done in the area of culture and disclosure rates, there are indications among child samples that children from minority groups face culture-specific barriers to disclosure that could contribute to delays or denials (Dunkerley & Dalenberg, 1999; Elliott & Briere, 1994; London et al., 2005, p. 205).
■ Severity and duration of abuse: Research stud- ies show inconsistent results. Future multivariate analyses accounting for severity and duration of abuse, age, gender, culture, and relationship to perpetrator may clarify this issue.
■ Batterers: The courts should be especially alert to the potential for child sexual abuse by batterers, because research studies indicate that battering father-figures are from four to nine times more likely to perpetrate incest (primarily on girls) than are non-batterers (Bancroft & Silverman, 2002). Because of the atmosphere of terror that can per- meate violent homes, both adult and child victims are often justifiably reluctant to speak up when formally questioned unless they can be convinced that they will not be in danger for doing so (Jaffe & Geffner, 1998).
■ Dissociation and post-traumatic stress: Children subjected to prolonged, severe abuse may face multiple obstacles to adequate disclosure. Unwillingness to face the discomfort of post-trau- matic flashbacks may cause traumatized children to numb their feelings and cognitions and shut down during interviews. Dissociative symptoms may interfere (Chaffin et al., 1997; Putnam, 1997). Cognitive disabilities caused by damage to the central nervous system and brain are associated in numerous studies with histories of severe child maltreatment in early childhood, and these deficits may interfere with children’s ability to recall and describe their life experiences (Elliott & Briere, 1994; Putnam, this issue).
■ Modesty: Modesty or embarrassment should also be considered as motives for silence. One labora- tory study indicates that girls aged 5-7 are reluc- tant to disclose even non-abusive genital touching during interviews. Saywitz, Goodman, Nicholas, and Moan (1991) found a 64% false negative dis- closure rate in a subsequent interview among girls who had been touched genitally and anally during
a pediatric examination. It was only when the girls were directly asked with a yes-or-no question if the doctor had touched them on the genital and anal areas that these girls disclosed. This sugges- tive question produced a false positive rate of 8% (three girls) among those in the control group who had not been genitally and anally touched, and one of these girls provided contextual details. Most experts in the field warn against interview questions that name both act and perpetrator, and many courts define such questions as leading. Nevertheless, in this study, there were eight times as many false denials when this suggestive ques- tion was not asked than there were false allega- tions when it was asked.
■ Other reasons for non-disclosure: When non-dis- closing sexually abused children are questioned, they cite fear as their primary motivation not to tell. Older children who are familiar with depen- dency procedures know that they and their sib- lings may be removed from their home if they tell. Children may fear being stigmatized as “sluts” or “faggots” by their schoolmates if word gets out (and it too often does) that they are sexual abuse victims. Children may fear consequences to themselves, to the perpetrator, or to other family members (Goodman-Brown et al., 2003). Children often otherwise love and trust sexual abuse perpe- trators, and in some cases, they may not be fully aware that what is happening to them is abusive, criminal, and wrong.
Conclusions The most difficult form of abuse to prove in court
is child sexual abuse, even in dependency cases where
the burden of proof is preponderance of the evidence
or clear and convincing evidence rather than proof
beyond a reasonable doubt. Few convictions carry the
same degree of stigma and legal ramifications for the
convicted and the potential for serious emotional and
psychological harm to the victim.
It is important to understand that the rules are
different in sexual abuse cases, and every judge must
understand the science. It is common in sexual abuse
cases for the victim not to disclose in a timely manner.
It is not unusual for the victim to disclose little by little
over a period of time. It can happen that the child victim
will recant. In any other prosecution for any other crime,
these actions would be considered indicia of unreliability
E r n a O l a f s o n a n d J u d g e C i n d y S . L e d e r m a n
37W i n t e r 2 0 0 6 • J u v e n i l e a n d F a m i l y C o u r t J o u r n a l
or lack of truthfulness and would be legal and factual
impediments to conviction. Indeed, a denial of abuse by
the alleged victim would prevent prosecution.
In dependency cases, the court is bound to protect
the health and safety of the child while balancing the
rights of the parents. It is important that judges under-
stand the science so that they can do justice when the
defense lawyer argues, “It did not happen because the
child recanted”; “It did not happen because the child’s
disclosures were not made close to the event”; “It did
not happen because the child kept adding new infor-
mation.” As in domestic violence, the often frustrating
behavior of the victim needs to be explained to the
trier of fact from the victim’s perspective, by those
who have studied this behavior.
When justice is not done in a sexual abuse case, the
harm can be devastating. No jurist wants to take a child
from her home and break up a family when abuse has not
occurred. No jurist wants to leave a child unprotected in
an abusive family. The reality is that it is very often diffi-
cult for a judge presiding over a child sexual abuse case to
feel certain about his or her decision and interpretation
of the facts. Many judges spend sleepless nights worrying
about the ramifications of their decisions.
S U M M A R Y O F R E S E A R C H F I N D I N G S
1. Experts agree that a majority of child sexual abuse victims do not disclose their abuse during childhood.
2. Experts agree that when children do disclose sexual abuse during childhood, it is often after long delays.
3. Prior disclosure predicts disclosure during formal interviews. Children who have told someone about the abuse prior to the formal interview are more likely to disclose during that interview than children who have not. Children who have not previously disclosed and who have come to the attention of the authorities because of medical evidence, videotapes, and other external evidence, are less likely to disclose during medical or investigative interviews than are previously disclosing children.
4. Gradual or incremental disclosure of child sexual abuse occurs in many cases, so that more than one interview may become necessary.
5. Experts disagree about whether children disclose sexual abuse when they are interviewed. However, when both suspicion bias and substantiation bias are factored out of studies, studies with external corroborating evidence of child sexual abuse show that 42% to 50% of children do not disclose sexual abuse when asked during formal interviews.
6. School-age children who do disclose are most likely to first tell a caregiver about what has happened to them.
7. Children first abused as adolescents are more likely to disclose than are younger children, and they are more likely to confide first in another adolescent than to a caregiver.
8. When children are asked why they did not tell about the sexual abuse, the most common answer is fear.
9. Further research is needed about recantation rates, which range in various studies from 4% to 22%.
10. Lack of maternal or parental support is a strong predictor of children’s denial of abuse during formal questioning. Abuse by a family member may inhibit disclosure. Dissociative and post-traumatic symp- toms may contribute to non-disclosure. Modesty, embarrassment, and stigmatization may contribute to non-disclosure. Gender, race, and ethnicity affect children’s disclosure patterns.
11. Many unanswered questions about children’s disclosure patterns remain, and further multivariate research is warranted.
38 J u v e n i l e a n d F a m i l y C o u r t J o u r n a l • W i n t e r 2 0 0 6
Sexual abuse cases are specialized cases that
require specialized knowledge, a tool judges must have
in order to do justice. Knowing the law alone is not
enough. By understanding the research in the sexual
abuse field (see page 37 for a summary of research
findings), judges can enhance their ability to make just
decisions by applying the law to the facts.
A U T H O R S ’ A D D R E S S E S :
Erna Olafson, Ph.D., Psy.D. The Childhood Trust
Department of Psychiatry, ML0539 University of Cincinnati Medical School
Cincinnati, OH 45267
Judge Cindy S. Lederman 11th Judicial Circuit Court
Juvenile Justice Center, Room 201 3300 N.W. 27th Avenue
Miami, FL 33142
C h i l d r e n ' s D i s c l o s u r e P a t t e r n s i n S e x u a l A b u s e C a s e s
E r n a O l a f s o n a n d J u d g e C i n d y S . L e d e r m a n
39W i n t e r 2 0 0 6 • J u v e n i l e a n d F a m i l y C o u r t J o u r n a l
Bancroft, L., & Silverman, J. G. (2002). The batterer as parent: Addressing the impact of domestic violence on family dynamics. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Bradley, A. R., & Wood, J. M. (1996). How do children tell? The disclosure process in child sexual abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 9, 881-891.
Carnes, C. N., Nelson-Gardell, D., Wilson, C., & Orgassa, U. C. (2001). Extended forensic evaluation when sexual abuse is suspected: A multisite field study. Child Maltreatment, 6(3), 230-242.
Carnes, C. N., Wilson, C., & Nelson-Gardell, D. (1999). Extended forensic evaluation when sexual abuse is suspected: A model and preliminary data. Child Maltreatment, 4(3), 242-254.
Chaffin, M., Lawson, L., Shelby, A., & Wherry, J. N. (1997). False negatives in sexual abuse interviews: Preliminary investigation of a relationship to dissociation. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 6(3), 15-29.
Dalenberg, C. J. (1996). Fantastic elements in child disclosure of abuse. APSAC Advisor, 9(2), 1, 5-10.
Dalenberg, C. J., Hyland, K. Z., & Cuevas, C. A. (2002). Sources of fantastic elements in allegations of abuse by adults and children. In M. Eisen, J. Quas & G. Goodman (Eds.), Memory and suggestibility in the forensic interview. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
DiPietro, E. K., Runyan, D. K., & Fredrickson, D. D. (1997). Predictors of disclosure during medical evaluation for suspected sexual abuse. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 6(1), 133-142.
Dubowitz, H., Black, M., & Harrington, D. (1992). The diagnosis of child sexual abuse. American Journal of Diseases of Children, 146(6), 688-693.
Dunkerley, G. K., & Dalenberg, C. J. (1999). Secret-keeping behaviors in black and white children as a function of interviewer race, racial identity, and risk for abuse. In K. Coulborn Faller & R. VanderLaan (Eds.), Maltreatment in early childhood: Tools for research-based intervention (pp. 13-36). New York, London, Oxford: The Haworth Maltreatment & Trauma Press.
Elliott, A. N., & Carnes, C. N. (2001). Reactions of nonoffending parents to the sexual abuse of their child: A review of the literature. Child Maltreatment, 6(4), 314-331.
Elliott, D. M., & Briere, J. (1994). Forensic sexual abuse evaluations of older children: Disclosures and symptomatology. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 12, 261-277.
Finkelhor, D., Hotaling, G., Lewis, I., & Smith, C. (1990). Sexual abuse in a national survey of adult men and women: Prevalence, characteristics, and risk factors. Child Abuse and Neglect, 14(1), 19-28.
Fivush, R. (1994). Young children’s event recall: Are memories constructed through discourse? Consciousness and Cognition, 3, 356-373.
Frasier, L. D., & Makoroff, K. L. (in press). Medical evidence and expert testimony in child sexual abuse. Juvenile and Family Court Journal, 57(1).
Ghetti, S., & Goodman, G. S. (2001). Resisting distortion? Psychologist, 14, 592–595.
Goodman-Brown, T. B., Edelstein, R. S., Goodman, G. S., Jones, D. P. H., & Gordon, D. S. (2003). Why children tell: A model of children’s disclosure of sexual abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 27, 525-540.
Henry, J. (1997). System intervention trauma to child sexual abuse victims following disclosure. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 12, 499-512.
Hershkowitz, I., Horowitz, D., & Lamb, M. E. (2005). Trends in children’s disclosure of abuse in Israel: A national study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 29(11), 1203-1214.
Jaffe, P., & Geffner, R. (1998). Child custody disputes and domestic violence: Critical issues in mental health, social service, and legal professionals. In G. Holden, R. Geffner & E. Jouriles (Eds.), Children exposed to marital violence: Theory, research, and applied issues (pp. 371-408). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Keary, K., & Fitzpatrick, C. (1994). Children’s disclosure of sexual abuse during formal investigation. Child Abuse and Neglect, 18(7), 543-548.
Kendall-Tackett, K. A., Williams, L. M., & Finkelhor, D. (1993). Impact of sexual abuse on children: A review and synthesis of recent empirical studies. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 164- 180.
Lamb, S., & Edgar-Smith, S. (1994). Aspects of disclosure: Mediators of outcome of childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9, 307-326.
Lawson, L., & Chaffin, M. (1992). False negatives in sexual abuse disclosure interviews: Incidence and influence of caretaker’s belief in abuse cases of accidental abuse discovery by diagnosis of STD. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 11, 107-117.
REFERENCES
C h i l d r e n ' s D i s c l o s u r e P a t t e r n s i n S e x u a l A b u s e C a s e s
40 J u v e n i l e a n d F a m i l y C o u r t J o u r n a l • W i n t e r 2 0 0 6
Levesque, J. (1994). Sex differences in the experience of child sexual victimization. Journal of Family Violence, 9, 357-369.
London, K., Bruck, M., Ceci, S. J., & Shuman, D. W. (2005). Disclosure of child sexual abuse: What does the research tell us about the ways that children tell? Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 11(1), 194-226.
London, K., Bruck, M., Ceci, S. J., & Shuman, D. W. (in press). Disclosure of child sexual abuse: A review of the contemporary empirical literature. In M. Pipe, M. Lamb, Y. Orbach & A. Cederborg (Eds.), Disclosing abuse: Delays, denials, retractions and incomplete accounts. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lyon, T. D. (2002). Scientific support for expert testimony on child sexual abuse accommodation. In J. R. Conte (Ed.), Critical issues in child sexual abuse (pp. 107-138). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Lyon, T. D. (in press). False denials: Overcoming biases in abuse disclosure research. In M. Pipe, M. Lamb, Y. Orbach & A. Cederborg (Eds.), Disclosing abuse: Delays, denials, retractions and incomplete accounts. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Malloy, L. C., Lyon, T. D., Quas, J. A., & Forman, J. (2005). Factors affecting children’s sexual abuse disclosure patterns in a social service sample. Paper presented at the American Psychological Society 17th Annual Convention, Los Angeles, CA.
Muram, D., Speck, P. M., & Gold, S. S. (1991). Genital abnormalities in female siblings and friends of child victims of sexual abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 15, 105-110.
The National Research Council (1993). Understanding Abuse and Neglect. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Olafson, E. (2002). When paradigms collide: Roland Summit and the rediscovery of child sexual abuse. In J. R. Conte (Ed.), Critical issues in child sexual abuse (pp. 71-106). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Pipe, M., Lamb, M., Orbach, Y., & Cederborg, A. (in press). Disclosing abuse: Delays, denials, retractions and incomplete accounts. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Poole, D. A., & Dickinson, J. J. (2005). The future of the protocol movement; Commentary on Hershkowitz, Horowitz, and Lamb (2005). Child Abuse and Neglect, 29, 1197-1202.
Putnam, F. W. (1997). Dissociation in Children and Adolescents. New York, London: The Guilford Press.
Putnam, F. W. (in press). The impact of trauma on child development. Juvenile and Family Court Journal, 57(1).
Sas, L. D., & Cunningham, A. H. (1995). Tipping the balance to tell the secret: The public discovery of child sexual abuse. London, Ontario, Canada: London Family Court Clinic.
Sauzier, M. (1989). Disclosure of child sexual abuse: For better or for worse. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 12, 455- 469.
Saywitz, K. J., Goodman, G. S., Nicholas, E., & Moan, S. F. (1991). Children’s memories of a physical examination involving genital touch: Implications for reports of child sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 682-691.
Sjoberg, R., & Lindblad, F. (2002). Delayed disclosure and disrupted communication during forensic investigation of child sexual abuse: A study of 47 corroborated cases. Acta Paediatrica, 91, 1391-1396.
Smith, D. W., Letourneau, E. J., Saunders, B. E., Kilpatrick, D. G., Resnick, H. S., & Best, C. L. (2000). Delay in disclosure of childhood rape: Results from a national survey. Child Abuse and Neglect, 24(2), 273-287.
Sorensen, T., & Snow, B. (1991). How children tell: The process of disclosure of child sexual abuse. Child Welfare, 70, 3-15.
Summit, R. C. (1983). The child sexual abuse accommodation syndrome. Child Abuse and Neglect, 7, 177-193.
Widom, C. S., & Morris, S. (1997). Accuracy of adult recollections of childhood victimization: Part 2. Child sexual abuse. Psychological Assessment, 9, 34-46.
REFERENCES
REDACTED STATEMENT: JANE DOE OCTOBER 2008 AGGRAVATED SEXUAL ASSAULT
I. \ txv e torn e -4\ne Pm -4-0 fy,c0/19 . (In 044; cAA (SW(Yncr4-
4/0 3i4lia1 Arleni&e._ 4n-4- criaat -An yve_ Line n U10.6 a 0.V. •
13-pnYie tij;AA,. it -tmeAtc;i1v4, "POtAcr&Pni ebe
"T.." onulti emdie rA t li EATit-oniunt- ke_S-f 64- -0/1( Office of the Attorney general.
Mir "dile
SIGNED:
VITNESS: TRANSCRIBED BY:
N c, New
+)n-b\ seM ekojs -The Ars+
Mai ea kte.s,.scs
(4,; n k. v../ 4 \Th0A • s .%-C-Ctrer4-
oMiess aw. xits-6
WVItct NC- ) +IVVUOV‘. qt nn."6"-- A inCk4
\ \VA ax\K tincte ti".14Cct\‘`l —vine se( .4.,,nck cackle-cc $
"PncV .izc o16c. 0„ka te.kc s
--Pck.A- \\FCC tr),6 =. \\veA ,1401AAn XNArriAltr
Sor t
08/03/2007 14:30
PAGE 29
STATEMENT OF: CR# PAGE: S
DATE: 01-66 -047 STARTING TIME: 0 ENDING TIME: j2,7"/ TAKEN BY: LOCATION: ilmimis
WITNESS:*411a; SIGNED: _iuwsop____ TRANSCRIBED BY:
4PAGE:STATEMENT OF: DATE: 0 -CS tY(7 STARTING TIME: j0:06# TAKEN DV:
ENDING TIME: ga7
LOCATIONS.
WITNESS.
WITNESS: tRANSCRD3ED IW:
SIGNED:
08/03/2007
PAGE 30
r no v -At ;— Nov c n4). t r- 04 Ickgrl.
t. tok, Dna cri e 111111110VaOmAcm vyn nv-e A \neseirlir"
"N \ni e trov-e -n-Knyy,
tt he- rt utftez for -v.\ -tinn ker 4rcr.
p-ko U-k-
o-i-Nne , ; c -Patag r- irrni thni NrnA °Aso AvNk tthnAir itnPP. 4Th6)S R
WITNESS:
SIGNED:
TRANSCRIBED BY;
08/03/2007 14:30 IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIMD PAGE 31
STATEMENT OF: PAGE: 5
DATE: E76:eil STARTING TIME: gage' ENDING ITME: 1.2-:299,6W
TAICEN BY: sOlegaln
41. • • IL la •
\ mos.\ CaS OAcsuce &KW- cod
\lorne C
4a5r w
LOCATIONIS
6") --C. r ( 3.101"-C ads eArk cks '001
—n? cr cr. %Nilo\ Nca ,ec. M lep.ts ovt.
‘,36 int(' LOCAL, abt t.ce elriaa3Zak-r--ad
We lt\ . CA) "IVe othuse )tiVacriv.Tres( i
iNntA Ate. t I
1 C.-Pr 3/41e Nnovwc e eve___12-1-29)-saa-\RS- d: a SM.. a• • "a_ -
Sfrisies1 • Clouse nr.ropre. urv6:1 -14 n erf--
‘rAisej . woo otiwt1/441 a y. cl_
\ent_ L)nn1 04po Nt. \\Ala( ari\k PAti Sl&ke/CS
08/03/2007 14:30
PAGE 32
STATEMENT OF.11111.111.1111. CRS PAGE: Q DATE: 046 -091 STARTING TIME: ltrObleil ENDING TIME: ge21/3/7 TAKEN BY: LOCATION: a
Ns a ^Fe sjnon)ec- misCek-Onte 000t Ise fly uhenevor- 4.11frawr- t th ).11)4- Oat* PA. .
_ a • 44
‘01 the -1-a\me sa ton c lyn Nanks 1/1.911. ckbosie)
tn -65e -Ly4t, Tak. 6;y4A1, werAt --cn (Th. erase *v. Thits,ln 12:"'"‘ nnut-t ) JeArcii vIcoq- \news - %testy\ (AY\ "MrAnt.T" ok I lot*
9).)1 Y•nagetAAN
SIGN/ED: alit— TRANSCRIBED BY:
08/03/2007 14:3 WSPD PAGE 33
••• •I•L•t• • ,
e nerga atanol- hemecre,_wsttex)D kr-
WITNESS!
!STATEMENT OF: PAGE: n IDATE; -0(7 STARTING TIME: aneof ENDING TIME: 42-,,,9,47 !TAKEN BY: LOCATION: 11111111MMED
1; cv ?eStinn° "Vvzseete reanner IANN. Apar \-ntnO\ rive (ber‘ _IsnriAj\tt YYlt tic Sn lrCkY1 .
vRwar Nrc‘ 4t'"na "4‘ tt At) \ 4)(Vot‘ 1-Nen -301.w.ers6.44-- kon 1•C\ iEv! V•Vs 0411.61 O‘Yla_ e Ins\NnTilt elenla _
Nnat „Az \ner eirm
-n" ow, wn4-. itte. -ftryle
tuntat 0)- wpote —Ng -itp.s1-- u 4.er vv.) rrott.0-r- e Myr clAAti-t-
t .nnv.Ac -pm ts\ry..• -IV'S 1:SC(4i C I /kW..." at, • • ,
r4‘‘),_ g‘s-Aer- -tLA mit_ 01.re nrxd) .StsNm • /v.& -aNnY... C, JS rn AsSc -kr) -S•ty
tfiptc SA1Dt4.) ‘-ve \real -to ijttic1
Peaer 04cpc4.4" rcis tk kJ) ( herds C 44 44‘13
Pne
SIGNED: 411/a11111111111.—
TRANSCRIBED BY:
‘o\u s-ncz,․) Sc ").o;\ istmi tak ) we. TvA. mnetiz c- A‘crc1/4__. noIA- SSA( Vvick -ft) pe t 11A-A sz LAW t 64.42 ‘2":5 Z. yNS kk e IC:avnetiter
Pnermrt4-- 1mA `9.€ &kJ, "isinvw c- 4-i-ktv.On -17)
Lk) tittt.)
-11-2) 40‘ r"
sister
08/03/2007 14:30 PAGE 34
STATEMENT OF: PAGE: -48
DATE: 0 -65-
Igoe
041 STARTING TIME: . ENDING TIME:a:27a,
TAKEN BY: LOCAT/ON:
EN.1\N_ \'c +an 1eoN4' t t.thS -irin sr -kcA \na. r. _OvIA gym twist r 4 ahcren
cackveim Nott roe tivit t 3ot 4- Tf \MAI kc3è W n
-Mt arsa, '.*AciAtvx+- fl tkotrfteMAtt.._ t tAint ti\ &u e\- (tnntr.&tt &C..s3cA a wens erv4 . evc
intik. virus. %.1 nnt Conik. Sims c‘at. NtArtne&sratC- way-6\et -Can r NO, leelnosibta scmt
SIGNED: 411111111111111.-- WITNESS: TRANSCRIBED BY:
AS CLIMIDSWV/11. :SVALCLIAt
—11111111S :GUMS :SSULLIM SCI`r3
Q
WO- a a) U17) it)
airy); e 1/4M
-}"Dik-% vP6v0 05- 311`)• ‘A")*(")1 CPI AA c?' vo t C"op HE N gu1/4 ‘vi rick) \1SO.A.
NOloct. ?wick ) 1A C,A s.,i4a WTI 9'
\Nip \two
tva? L \1/4t 1 V(YI VUM C\kt I 5 k eitio '-u-
'°1' lz)e k ‘03/4 IR ?VW Lir) n u) Agt-TI,\
ritti ass I dfl ) ;et !-)PAXX n pen 9- ‘N% 6-0 OVICI t.0
)\1031- (41" 1-Skiirn at9Q‘KA LAx VW0 on rp.? —m—ren---rrallarws-4 \ant:
4 -MI_ -4.71M \fle.
2140In301 /4331Y1
DATIONZ ao staurvis 49-0 :aava
104 :3Dva :30
-YardimenmatS SO 1- i() Tran.:.; y4,
0111.1111111S1111.11. E/E0/88SE 39tod
1
WITNESS:
08/83/2007 14:30 SD
PAGE 36
STATEMENT OF
Da-046 -17
TAKEN BY:
a ILI • Itai iiitat.t.r.-s:AMBe a I
STARTING TIME:
PAGE: lb ENDING TIME:
LOCATION:a
*'•/
--Ns "et n it -4.1(1%0i 0n05_ (ter ‘NncikerrC--- t.
4t\ -1r'sciftx.)1
C PtVle
OAP vvmi tett- WO. C6111C
\.g CKI n't& my) hays 11* (1.114 Cumvple
-1/2horin0A WV? •=. SO.\ k WC) . \-\C
ntt ObucAbni
_.ictnon ak eke eve le•
so,‘ X v :ha+
schrea 1 an A -c -in Nct NI tin 4itivre kg/ ee. Ain would,. \hutmertattr•
SAIn &Iv\ kihr -Stki 0A.1 ecri-
--‘nue)r. onrvi 5t- IA* tatMLci rYK. ark 1ump . rin Mk/ buit, ANA The tirre-fo ultwel • he rnia (malt Stitt, VT) ir.hx4-
—Mk vm 44- Vie- LOPYth- --b., 4110 SChn())
lt"A+ PAC o'N\ nni 1/2 flax
I. SIGNED:a111119—
WITNESS: TRANSCRIBED BY:
.10 liAs
08/03/2007 14:30
PAGE 37
STATEMENT OF: CRit PAGE:
DATE: 00 -03 -eel STARTING TIME: toin ENDING TIME: a
TAKEN BY: qm LOCATION: 111111111111111111.
See u yks 4trm ikA \net, 5\14' (An --k1C11\41.-
c\U tvn‘&5 (*CNC (1/4-'1nck jr?\k& w+tz. An EV VIV OKI *INA n .40 ton kaic 4ksi beleS out. ktibutk
eVon -PD•SA- C N0e04- etkif t net ,
Wreka, tut In I Le I ma.) ,sh torstt--- Vint In-E- 4142.`A 5.04. (hi. 4-11/4) --i-hnne 4 7-41‘,/ In 0-4- 'Or s-c-c nig A +IV nivy, 9 00;- r1, ht 044442._
et a A Nita 'dim l a O cL4f1n 'j an&
k+III Sri, WI. O. `ft\ INeN5, n4\14 fc vfairt te.. 1 -4A-Ektg._ execp..)-"
ny,,c ts, „„,ADA -A, a erre c-k- Ott-ce
reNkii \CO'brkYN r
eanowN, c' \ A— sAe. LION.) Q/Vt•PAL -1/27'N tOtvnit OkYkA, 1344 'hu innutk pre-it ryi_
ann A MS dee p tTh flCVfr ate /I ATti ele es
WITNESS: SIGNED:
WITNESS:
TRANSCRIBED BY:
88/83/2807 1
PAGE 38
STATEMENT OF: CR# PAGE:
DATE: 0-6 -047 STARTING TIME: V ENDING TIME: ardep,'
TAKEN BY: p LOCATION: eallIMMO
k I AYst )\ ni>1/4A\ ni-soN cwj SYCyltal) atkri 0 INA c r (1,4— rve.ALkomA netvim i -4.ent L
lihaeD W CAW )\C\ +nit nn INA WO I* -W14 1,:a 3 oak ci
rnA") reni Nelmsevy, a ft 01/4 . EV4 ki IN\SWk- V14 + tArk ‘I" Vurks̀ c CM14 •
-Ts. -4 r•rat 4...tsti.Sre y khp_sk INN \last, 4\ Occtek \MI Firk.“ k- whkna az -tv,.“ kiwi; k \tvt c:s ct )\n,41. n::4-.(v) \IN nk tan On trra, astskT- Vat err Orbit
ett
Arc\ taw" \nit C 'ykt t NAP A \irst Ail eVri ‘Ak‘IL IAA ar LOVA CiAtt • %rye ak\niP c
Lunt L actie_wi se e NeN\W• Ar1 hit I twit) 1.1( Imni4 -kel\ cts 4r Lotto 4) . M t oNTE. Mit r
rel 44Pstot tont A rspit, *-stArsaa- \Nie AAVN'A-• \L4 c \Lb W\ Itze.m. "nd$ 1.4 MU 1k
WITNESS: SIGNEDe- WITNESS: TRANSCRIBED BY:
08/03/2007 14:30
SIGNED:
TRANSCRIBED Int
PAGE 39
WITNESS:
STATEMENT OF. CR# PAGE: /5
DATE: lat -6 -0, STARTING TIME: idte ENDING TIME: aratAV TAKEN BY: LOCATION: a
• . toa'rk- 411 (\NI c tiskt. Ink (IMP° le ONA., bt-1- of (DIA Carrie in -01.it
VNWS R-4101Y\ Ap mg vet1,t)t NtAvnAlk i ckl*-No I4I ok
enue‘e aetv.in trIr in 60v A nse\ ni-to -r -kinnyk- ujixAk newa n 11-t
Covl C .flyne exr4 Oust now • IA INS. LA) tz-cteN e:3‘441- thVerr-n LtreN\AO 17ktt
—b flrw kb. ) \ANN& \e" €7,0% ° Yvkli 'ism)* \
S.to r IN(SS2 a br.syrk- Alt\ Itel Cu )
IN\ ki .C nAske 9- yv %Or ujiNG ),C) _S•1 -e \It 1.04(
ot. bA4-47-tyy. b <NI
• 0 Nt a,C1) S\111 Nui —77 sDA- -46 Yi\iikt raink
rtrAtse isoms3/4;ne,fick4
sister
WITNESS:
STATEMENT OF: CRF PAGE:
DATE: 01-6 -047 Siall.111.111,ARTING TIME: GM'
.
LOCATION: isENDING TIME: 14°:TAKEN BY:
WITNESS:
08183/200ealliMe
PAGE 40
. is cl
-kfitog britAts ht. nfitu btel sister rep — fithrs-021 e Saw Ahn. rna-44
Weil -3' an ti: the v y C-tmir-M slit, A
.....tar—All—tig.--IstakALL-)rnt; h itt i-te . 1-1 n Sa j1/4. Snatt flat ton m. ; ttio4—
LUNA* \ALI- ira \\L4 \f‘Neyt Um, • 1.4 94. -Setsue. The u..tre+ 4n
Ant\ nnThvnierk rk4TAA NVA Aelf
.X -it 101. r 5ive44 hu-nitt,4 sods__ Lentos' at-- 11.• tient wife --Hut 5-huni rit4 -Me rnas+ rh.t.
-VAS r ite" }wit' bottrl re
s ussmnil— hn otit
s'a '4-
Nn
4 -4.-1-$44. mscfr
SIG
TRANSCRIBED BY:
etrivon
at /fl
vnTNESS:
WITNESS: 1 'Tr AI Sr SIGNED. WITNESS: TRANSCRIBED BY
08/03/2007
PAGE 41
STATEMENT OF: CR# PAGE: I; DATE: 0146 -0€.7 STARTING TIME: .164W ENDING TIME: 17,709/0/ TAICEN BY: ••n11111n•• LOCATION: a
; fl. tu4 1.-no tyt. lit rile
ektho m , ic 641, N414c1 A V,' JAye 0-r-Hvz.
cowl, . He 4n41 n. iste_ ot C/c1fssC cold hi t e ci or nit rno sarktv`k d Ord he-
t. la3 60.,sy pet rkV II 1Ynan Cot/ \err
814 on_daf t-a'n • cy-6 (At 'VW IN-( .14- VAIL 9N14 1 ar 3wri- irme ;AA d Iss‘w\ +n
het rne 62ost Vst %A i 41 int , 44- —11,4t-1- 44-nt n b4;tvid -5-, ( . 6 trt 64/tv ttri arr-
......Az_cittScal_be tem St hotti PA 6A re rse4- CAM nk • .-.40ka NC- 5/44-11t 0, a tannri w
lys3 if. 4- MX- 44.2 hunt 0.), a bak -in 44- kiw' tk
08/03/2007
PAGE 42
STATEMENT OF: CM PAGE: t6 DATE: 0 -05 -ø' STARTING TIME: alga ENDING TIME:a272),7 TAKEN BY: LOCATION: a
my e_ a i,us,d All The (Ali -OlintA tan( 0,79 maniA wets Aome voor id IAA /4- 1417 Ck JAMS Nen -Hkt pAn-ut nee: woe (1134farnnrri41 he Ci#416 .6r 4101/44) @ rfilt Mot v
hy- &vie Ihti niMist pri4- u.S >n -N 411 L Iliasand sr% d n so •"1 -1-Aft
i\n3 born And 544- an tAIL ?kyle. my g_ boould Inaci. --6
4k \-tMq_cry, 4.1c tanod "ui4i4i rv ?ha4c Li 4 s 0.1 tll ii vAi- pun Ilut ChnveL cu rt Cintka kit 1Crid 3cre1/4,_
piNkr n h wcvi Lorna %Fe. 13-4- kra-tni-omn nor.2. eiczts -4-1. a (ken 12_ . W t It& Athrv kir _bt _Sn'Si Srkr1111111. .ntiltitc4
n4' (ALM os\A tnt uwika 4e t\
SIGNED: SEP--
TRANSOMED BY:
I • • • A WA'
WITNESS:
08/03/2007 14:38 .1.111110111111111. PAGE 43
STATEMENT OF: CR* PAGE: 17
DATE: 0-13 -,7 STARTING TIME: A 0Itfl ENDING TIME: ajgreg TAKEN BY: LOCATION: itallarillia
iAnd -ItorN ovcskeledie RSCIlc iiond -+if Le a
si-fse-le A (P-4aCtIree3 th6± 14 31a4f.A.eci S alsister _.,;„N
cuinck- atrwic., itt vitnOtk it IELIMII-fn wit- ni• -AA) nnvt eyo_ Sioh t to/ lisp at-I-flown. fiC
hie ow ti t so‘li thIs‘en +k tAThtiI 't1\ Va Te n it\ 0M _crittur new VA z`nA nTres
tic thnu.&h tk- rAi4- rns1 _A s ksho Ca -A:A -Acre( 6e. ro_usse_ Inc Altai oa rAp -4a be. 04144
nattste._ %Amin& Ias, trN -Mk FlArrek ;10
4) On 10,10- caati cs."
INNI K.A.e Ion 4tr4 cot) (Er 'fits+ -tit
SIGNED:
'ERANSCRESED BY:
PAGE 44 08/03/2007 14:30
STATEMENT OF: CRO PAGE: r3
DATEz-0 -0113 - 017 STARTING TIME: IL ENDING TIM:a:20W
TAKEN BY: LOCATION: (S
use
1st _IA its het- (knit irad , Fre, (0 ARA__
1:444-sat4bbv. anti 41.6%8 it4- nu, to he nw 9h. (C
MA? inlln n Cams na-1- +k I itchItoron cc- e erni
c 14- 4sticA4c. A4+ ot gaia '1St( (NM_ ct i‘e knet.s cims >es -4.1 vnv.s-epirh,
OkiCip44 ‘Net.t tws-ks 0,,c4 -in _suck_ A
SA : Cnvna ed An NI Anti, jw4Se4 nAl ke0li4 ciovAQ skerAta 4 rymitraf- kiN rmi nics44, and
vms g1M1\1\S bYX +hp -Pte-nt ate\ •frkt Cfrwet-c. 'QAI
ekr. Arsai flys_ "It) ' 44. AittA\ nu) .1 \-% linkn V\
teattrt -17, +11 0-win - IN/ CR 113E-- \erre CO
rtt ottA Sict: A qiii‘n/liC it/IPS \I/CA • 014 1111111.24-
: WITNESS: SIGNED:
ATTNESS: TRANSCRIBED BY:
WITNESS:
VVTINESS:
08/03/2007 14:30
PACE 45
STATEMENT OF: CRit PAGE: sg DATE: 0 ege,,-09" STARTING TIME: ." ENDING TIME: g.29947 TAKEN BY: LOCATION:
Sa t 1 I Car\ nut 7t1- \\ ins J'ew caà tvn ewe enki Alm+ snA nts e *Ante S.ovitrf bs
pJ3of5 ynti , X- a s.; c K -4 t4- acc
%Ai n Thapbs 4, Von, al\ A Cs- Ye irk
PACT*U P ot)lk 1;..e rkvyti
As q OtNtAa_ oiOtia- 4H+ cl-r th ilk jaUxiat. ‘N../ciaucts Citft c. 11/4flhAlva-
Linnet WS 00. 144-it n,ilt en inss4(o trn
Az pitsgibu. . NNInAne‘ m ‘...>Ath km4- A4IN Ytt\W e. fkk O. C I; ‘ k 'In eta\ k tel rtaltbaft- -11inark- en
A4N1/4. et r.1‘\\X Vann"1\3 ‘a- caw0 "Cf-on\ fa CO / 3/4 CC) —SbOcrli.61.---MatokereaL-SIS.L....3212401SSICAS,
t ke CAtA VV.A ) rAntt, ty a wolc-4- \ton/ Svely-4- \-4
skulk 1:;thi ivnor , it tALLP .51n Itie —p Ock Meat
SIGNED:
. cØrssasst_
TRANSCRIBED BY:
Sas PAGE 46,08/03/2007 14:36
CM/ PAGE: 9 DATE: -.6 STARTING TIME: Invivfie7 ENDING TIME: ef:nen STATEMENT OF: WarMaire TAKEN BY: n•n11•1
LOCATION:
hV\--enakoT UnEXM aujuk -}e wte -1604_ tio •
4-0 &t, Ark- uatAkik 4R2, thm hone 4-1 11M i$*' 47, wa1/41, u.3 1-4e
£4 ' • t. fl. " L2
eAt t-pwss 00 .1- .
v tea t via nee A Lc riy 1re/a 4-6 abuse_
\No Ittn-k- SINnit / e S fk th It e "Asir \no tx \ NA -C^^ ,Nni 4 *IV tint- AC ‘c-� Mist:vitt nor 1a SNYYksit-h\-ns 4YLTInn "KA_
hi raflflrL tkc OIL EWA cs)eg 41\ Y1/44._ rytarAo (In o er-Erfür rn csrtj .S.,� me%
1-N N'YI /AM; \t a Scv4 nI flt e On r fre 114, werIA-- 5,24- sits.ts ‘ ckNA. Asiu5 Aeoje
-S&L) tit \‘'N %WO- CM- (ilea iinAin14 SC 4 Orve \At (kovt.V voktA twserm i4 n-s
SIGNED: ellIMM!FWITNESS: WITNESS: TRANSCRIBED BY:
r)r sko
08/03/2087 PAGE 47
STATEMENT OF: CRS PAGE:
DATE: 0 -6 -17 STARTING TIME: i t ENDING TIME: a t_ni'W TAKEN BY: LOCATION: a
_120�,a&L:LL-
Abet O. sip t 4,1 VA4.1‘ 6.1 C ntV1 • -C-± \LIN> ntkl.- Ot NIVZ. led S rent Anmli ny- ryd.Vt mit 611..t ,n cif614 ikrstN- clox i i‘A rxfl ci .on? *44 KR
ArA dti pet, dt, ind Nut perfron ova( • re 4-1 714._ ,. • to& a t •
164- (4- _frio en ;orb 44cici,\ VISA- Wks IA lush' Vi-
/h9 A4p‘o Akke Sorm enOto c pie_ nv44- no3 -bk
muck one_ c ;4- Ix> Jek 1/4
44, ?Afire be4- IkD 714, I 1 cet
,Ag tart sOn -(-kg
isick‘k, Lo 4, -11,4
.N41 lteANC
rpreAets *love rim tocveng.
SIGNED:_.
WITNESS: TRANSCRIBED BY:
STATEMENT OF:
DATE: 2E6 -z:trZ. STARTING TIME: TAKEN BY:
PAGE:
ENDING TIME: 47.:ZZon LOCATION: a41•101•n••
08/83/2087 PACE 48
Qr9
1\1\viN t4i roo4itir- ivatd Air yth (LEW ws ha nu 6.1 har4- de-Act- nv\ A t(
Wort etwral lk. ft nifirbsel fit- tm--- 71C- Y... hNisin12n4e- €:403 s44 nmf cis4ic
Sts
hit ryi , V%41 to + -0.0; + Oats --p- 7w siykk smelt
-c4 C""t \rame"N (IAA 614- kesy: +by on rt..,
Y.INND Pakr INAttfw e. '1)14- kw ly3 (10.4 ni b1/4/4- tpt, rAi c4- b-• YNti nirtbeir pimt
43 Iva rnotv c vs\ ret4.Sytirkt .11.1 y\ IvoNi tac \'‘e tOrti tXMA-- 0414-- 40 jctcncw ik(GOI
it 4Pok tat rnmv4 d 111111) 4.
Av't LAM C.
litc-i- ni katitly ni TrWa-le erre t ON purpt,Se Plito.NA 4CKIA
k writ_ vik ifrciN ,p.4.0,va, WITNESS* SIGNED: 4011111.111111111_ WITNESS: TRANSCRIBED BY:
4 '4 ' iviy5
azaniastivitt atom :smut%
:micas
_pram4 5t4fcQO4l .tv isivephos, (Tim*
rAV I" k "ir • 'I* • • •••/ •
Insta) ylk I dtt M143 %kJ-- art dam) P...‘ 4J,\ --1-017 TIN In (MC aijJl pAv onito rAilu-10
Tiomp\pvir, 741440L Pw? ti‘ WV\ 141.mcf 13 v5 1-f Flew -1":u r 111 -nos 1'TV
jrd'et0 d Jtifftq%t+ 1119 C7-VINI) Ct CI" L1/41 11,0‘14-1 "S-- ( De -t."fl 1RU Va 9S t rik t4v1
a-6o tu. v--0 rimylets -14-43, at:LA of- -1-vt lug
3v.p %um 794. I I" et's 034-
rrFnnr-+rr+!nn ei-i^ WO Nea1/4\ -V; • k.194_ WOCA-1 k1/4Vt -1-30‘ ?Tr
Ce:NOLLV301 Zaeltejra:alial. ONIGNIa
_e-c :30Vd
aft
:314ILL sta.urvis 49- yap anfa :AO INKIMILVLS
6t7 39Vd OE :PI LUIZ /EOM
WITNESS:
WITNESS:
88/03/2007
PAGE 50
kit)
23 STATEMENT OF: CItti PAGE:
DATE: -6 -0(7 STARTING TIME: 44 ENDING TIME: /21200*
TAKEN BY: LOCATION: le
-1-71 he until y#u' lake,
nwk hoA 44 ov. tthrv, an-I
1-s t orfecrtmAi Ctoreua_ itt es 441.'4- oh ‘NcIrao 1„,t34--le CCN're 4-1/ C W 11 gel Ship' I)-(
es Iva* as hit tM. ni‘i k kk._ (14- rhtt bilA --a our est ANN& n hiot II+ wit 661INS rk Nzt hi t rn 6441- Mut( yrin ce
nl1 nns kAJ-t4t 6pson
"Ron" --170t ociumitift(WittL_sandit._ /
rAl COW 2.an yin 5L �ti wf Melo 4a SM1 MI ‘MQ 1/410 'lobo. 45 Kul, • rrv1 4ki4Q4 4,41AI
TLc( -he ) q 303+- 1). (Intel ram c toci /me re--
Qtpc. ‘tuinpowl
SIGNED.
'TRANSCRIBED BY:
08/83/2007 14:30 3367737994
WSPD
PAGE 51
STATEMENT OF: CR# PAGE:
DATE: 0-193 -017 STARTING TIME: itlitftil ENDING TIME: 1Z70W TAKEN BY: LOCATION: a
ta_�_salk__)2AThssitra L1OA s41:3.)AS 54- sl dc
•
cAnn\ ci n:k.
ek sorNeink WCt3i ict p A- (A se &&4-:
ty4 it t. \irteratil Se, anti., thstA- -nvki -1020 14,e_ Ail s, , rtM Klwitt nth-h -tn hmen... 7Inte-- fl J& )petttuic
14?we 044A-r `tyNCLIT tem uktire r koi been SO( ect4 'on &Mats ?1/4.3k —104 )iv fAl 111110 \at\ 0.1•A *AO te-le à rrakarat' t. Lot .
WTINESS:
SIG/NED: 4101111111111.--..—TRANSCRIBED BY:
08/03/2007 14:30 PAGE 52
WITNESS:
STATEMENT OF: CR# PAGE:
DATE:430 —OS —047 STARTING TIME: ged go, ENDING MX: AllOPity TAKEN BY: LOCATION: a
)64111rn Nti M ke ±Amsc wwq nkt kb CS141-" llt unlad
i4 sister
kat.444i. jut4- Ont. It I.0 4; A nti- wroirk, 47) b hoot&
.47) btkiN- 05 0 so, o 3lineick crn btinclador (ma t vinui6 4 hbafre -(71 5e+ mioniNra° ,
pk c4;r1 tars+k,„( 41 inuk loe St kkiv4 • cs nnA sa t ta itultk ipso rvioa. e inerek (re cks.
sk tia yen( chnss., ,A49 ries Pifr -1-A )y-e._ I ; ?c.c. imp yNci 11.111.kr__
Ovivk AlAN ANInX‘i betk'CAup SktA be pbo?._. 4fe kw 4 P4-1 194S r ks-Ie -fig
'fl.'ts 44. 1 - SGOZ O "A 0 4fti kNoi ..frvy MS. v.tto SeCtt1 . Ile c.41 -r-, 1.1/6> srna-4-
ContiMt4 ht kilicr rtrIMVIC 5n kr -r4 Artitut •
SIGNED:a-
TRANSCRIBED BY:
riviA r hevtjc- 40 id h3rIng r
SIGNED:
TRANSCRIBED BY:
08/03/2007
PAGE 53
2'
STATEMENT OF: CR# PAGE: a
DATE: 0-05-#7 STARTING TIME: M stg 411 ENDING TIME: alZereir TAKEN BY: LOCATION:
utgt L Beres° -1-tas -fs ICtIOW Tf. •
(otitci eknist- -44.2 M. -DA 'tar -11%*?ed- 5votk—r-S.4 5 n Ins -k 4th rul -AY hcl gsko .Ca-k- q pcirrs-
nip. »,..1 rintor .Si, --1iv&4- 4 r 9 Sa-i— nn
so. Ne *r# j4t flñdt. --;o1A tr.aWiermAt
\\bus kple ..-1-hrts, <1.0 wo,1/4 -theM nn
V4 Cd4.... k rdtkic 4o4.. rut \AIN./ qu i Coty:A-
Aar 11 \ ahritki h 47, Awfoc- 4:1) Pit COE_ -SC or-eic
c iatitc %14tfts 1 11 4c fl rr aci-e + So onacti-
h rkk n. 114- --Per)thA .Cifqr e
W \Iv lymstni. Nun @. qr .\ rad . ova
Ser6 vvvistarri r A .= cave:it A 4.. ittl keep
WITNESSI*4114
WITNESS:
14. ams 'aI tIla
11111111111111,— .SIGNED:TRANSCRIBED BY:WITNE%WITNESS:
Nay. a PAGE 54 g()
27 STATEMENT OF: CRF PAGE: skV
DATE: $-16 -t'7 STARTING TIME: ENDING TIME: 2.2f,o,
TAKEN BY: LOCATION:
11.1int magyi0. \nnu) vz-
as:Av. 14- a SSA' XIC 414.0 Ah it (Y10 6.11V3U tkilk
%sk \sen,ust &- -.
. jeLjible) A-11 j'h ILAA. s ykat
y,10 rf nht) 14.st cow r N-tAim sc Item%) A-C--Ut, rntlesb\rs rjCsn4 an4 kt qiiposeci
--N1 '4%.9 rn M itsit kg_ 0,k) 0.61SAL et n jahini,L4
s4a.. V•4 AVE og-A Ahock- k ainxtd kis nwp
• t • • •
A;A A.04- ktr,3 rmi 'PAY iket Ayouna 44t ro tit.r ;xi _its
eXth Brikt4 had r1/4 1at4M*1 pArt Ai -4-410
08/03/2007 14: PAGE 55
,ast know. Sk. ti ? ynel el • 7.-7 • kma jtn lerovi le 40 .04 cutren+
VS/ hi I it
STATEMENT OF: CRIF PAGE: 2-eir
DATE: -16 STARTING TIME: 15: Joo, ENDING TIME: awsew TAKEN BY: LOCATION: 111.111111a
-Arid *a lee tsrlilk (yds awl.. re4 Anws Sa.±es,t..326si__C?„6/Irlas Ino-v-e _11 e021/12P311-• 04 *ha+ '2v. if a 2 rvt--)
ci -tire NT 'TóVtac -him \tars as() --17, 3 fb.r1.- . Ant- 'Ainozsc- khoe..k rvv. i six Acs we ice -Inn se vet k ht sit 11"6 k • th
WITNESS:4dArei
SIGED• (411.1a WITNESS: TRANSCRIBED BY:
SIGNED:
TRANSCRIBED BY:
88/03/2007 14:30 a PAGE 56 C.3)
2t7 STATEMENT OF: CF# PAGE:
DATE: jaSei -017 STARTING TIME: 00 ENDING TIME: at29,207 TAKEN BY: LOCATION: Sal
4- NEM e_. occAncii 4, n,..,, o 14-1 1 -Vic
S 4 liNtratf LAMA .44.13 (cr45 \,-1545 n (\Nock Spiti44(53 Cycsta4-‘ (Ira MP t emerr; 01C.C. \WS 5) .1\1C/ea. bitY\ cleit tor el rin.St 1-g.
cirn S€ &t-4 pond .crfv4; km-k— *hot+ -}fil-1
‘ 1 --KIL 651*- ---15 4.o
t oruvuk enA jç,. ).%4 ski- Ozkre ()ANA Jo st ako. scC
3/4 i%,0 rt- Ann t1/4 A Ittow- A Uswihkir_IS
k `k
42) CiCtiCe c cit A 9cm entlyl iifti Jjp).A X1114.2. C Are
\,bre Cue 00 -4/CH-e1
eta Ns/1cl -kr-ri..s ituAAjt o andl "Mt* hks Ica -sem_ •l len+ 64 pv
On."
VICTIM IMPACT STATEMENT
Prosecutor's File #: (;) — Defendant's Name: ftf...tnala Indictment*:
In the space below, please write about how you and your lam* were affected by this incident. Please do not talk about the facts of the case or anything you might think of as testimony The judge and the prosecutor would like to know:
a. Your feelings about the incident b. How your life is different because of the incident c. What you think the defendant's sentence should be
Your Name:
WAS -itn-4,1e, (mei r ,1,; 41/4 .54.,nutt1/4. /xi-ye -In tzi.nA, UM*, 5Jt e-;nas.
1- WS itdel APA t ka5 matt f. tvi.e 4' +yre o-P rersnn tImet-1- s tnIne voxy 0A,14A. it-Avat :Atm,/ r.ew-Finas (AnA r Mier, -2r,r en 14 4— -rz Snme nint
i war, ar..n tip OY \ ryits k UN+ OSJCAMMtvl IC rin.( in up ryve SMCHLá 1st 1 ryi oat 0,1itAren a S e 14- 04 M; 1/4 SA 10 iiCnn •
%. •111. • • •••
scnit err. A krx )11
rAse. /ono tnns.i Ace Cornp, -1-60ec the r rirmr kArvt4 „arks , es no 4- hnotre The •-in An 6n 4- rts iNn ,t -1,;.t
• s 'nsibflji41 P -0Me %, 1 94. -In e nc,t) 012 a%
%AI
nos, . • fin 11. 1 S 01/4 (1,r. irsive . rE" 1411.1 rny Pespnns ;hi la .4eiLS.LLGese
#11 L- , ,
*-ib t -in ti4N and m artkirii I r -knelt,/ riel 44.p. eras?, ths *X Please return this form and all documents within 5 business days to:
If you have any questions, please call the County Office of Victim Witness Advocacy at
- ~OTF36BP000F.PDF.pdf
- Page 1
- Page 2
- Page 3
- Page 4
- Page 5
- Page 6
- ~OT8486P000F.PDF.pdf
- Page 1
- Page 2
- Page 3
- Page 4
- Page 5
- Page 6
- Page 7
- Page 8
- Page 9
- Page 10
- Page 11
- Page 12
- Page 13
- Page 14
- Page 15
- Page 16
- Page 17
- Page 18
- Page 19
- Page 20
- Page 21
- Page 22
- Page 23
- Page 24

Get help from top-rated tutors in any subject.
Efficiently complete your homework and academic assignments by getting help from the experts at homeworkarchive.com