[removed]

Fathers’ Influence on Children’s Cognitive and Behavioural Functioning: A Longitudinal Study of Canadian Families

Erin Pougnet, Lisa A. Serbin, Dale M. Stack, and Alex E. Schwartzman Concordia University

An emerging body of research illustrates the connections between fathers and their children’s develop- ment. This topic is particularly relevant in Québec, a demographically and culturally unique province in which female lone parenthood is relatively common; this pattern is related to socioeconomic disadvan- tages that predict negative cognitive and behavioural outcomes in youth. Using data from the Concordia Longitudinal Risk Project, an intergenerational longitudinal data set collected in inner city areas of Montreal, the current study investigated the prospective relations between fathers’ presence and parent- ing, and children’s subsequent cognitive and behavioural functioning. The current sample included 138 families from lower to middle income backgrounds who participated in two waves of data collection: when children were in middle childhood and subsequently three to five years later in preadolescence. The results indicated that for girls only, fathers’ presence in middle childhood predicted fewer internalizing problems in preadolescence. For both boys and girls, fathers’ positive parental control predicted higher Performance IQ and fewer internalizing problems over six years later. These findings add to the increasing body of literature suggesting that fathers make important contributions to their children’s cognitive and behavioural functioning, and point to the benefits of developing policies that encourage fathers to spend time with their children (i.e., parental leave for men) and promote positive fathering and involvement through parenting courses.

Keywords: fathers, behaviour problems, cognitive functioning, Canadian context, socioeconomic risk

One type of family that is becoming increasingly common in North America is the family headed by a single mother. In 2006, approximately 13% of Canadian families and 22% of families in Québec included biological fathers who lived apart from their children (Statistics Canada, 2007a). Families with one parent who does not live at home typically have lower income levels than two-parent families (Ricciuti, 2004). In 2006, 7.7% of Canadian children and youth in two-parent families fell below the low income cutoff, compared with 32.3% of children and youth in female lone-parent families (Statistics Canada, 2009). The devel- opmental outcomes of parental absence and low socioeconomic status (SES), including income level and educational attainment, are important to take into account in Québec, as research has indicated that Québec students who attend public schools in non- disadvantaged areas are one and a half times more likely to graduate high school than students attending schools in economi-

cally disadvantaged areas (Gouvernement du Québec, 2008). SES is related to indicators of success in adolescence, and one factor that is related to lower family SES is the absence of a parent from the home.

Most of the research involving families and child outcomes focuses on associations between mothers’ parenting and child development (Roy & Kwon, 2007). A growing minority of exist- ing research illustrates the associations between fathers’ presence; specifically, fathers living full-time in their biological children’s homes and child development. However, many researchers argue that fathers’ presence is not a detailed enough variable to under- stand children’s experiences (Flouri, 2007); thus, fathers’ parent- ing is often measured in conjunction with fathers’ presence in their children’s homes. In order to more closely examine the role of fathers in children’s development, the current study investigated the prospective associations between fathers’ presence and parent- ing and children’s cognitive and behavioural functioning in an urban French Canadian context.

In general, studies outside of Canada have demonstrated that fathers’ presence in their children’s homes and parenting are positively associated with children’s cognitive outcomes across time after controlling for various demographic and socioeconomic factors (for review, see Allen & Daly, 2002). The majority of the recent fathering research has been conducted with preschool-aged children. For example, a study conducted in the United States by Ryan, Martin, and Brooks-Gunn (2006) found that children who lived with two highly supportive parents at 2-years-old had higher Bayley Scales of Infant Development-II (Bayley, 1993) Mental Development Index scores (reflecting cognitive and language de- velopment) at 3 years of age than children who lived with one or

Erin Pougnet, Lisa A. Serbin, Dale M. Stack, and Alex E. Schwartzman, Centre for Research in Human Development, Concordia University.

The preparation of this article was supported by an award from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, as well as grants from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (6070-10-5/9515, 410-99-1415, and 410-2002-1279). We thank Claude Senneville, Ming Wang, Alexa Martin-Storey, Lina Saldarriaga, and the Concordia Project team for their assistance in data collection and multiple imputation meth- ods. Moreover, we are most indebted to the participants in the study.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Erin Pougnet, Centre for Research in Human Development, Concordia Univer- sity, 7141 Sherbrooke Street West, Montreal, Québec H4B 1R6, Canada. E-mail: [email protected]

Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science © 2011 Canadian Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 43, No. 3, 173–182 0008-400X/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0023948

173

no supportive parents after controlling for SES. Studies conducted with the older offspring of absent fathers have supported these findings. For example, a study conducted in the United States indicated that children whose fathers lived with them full-time had higher scores on reading and math tests than children whose fathers did not live with them (Teachman, Day, Paasch, Carver, & Call, 1998). Moreover, paternal parenting has been shown to be positively associated with children’s cognitive outcomes for dif- ferent age groups (Bronte-Tinkew, Carrano, Horowitz, & Kinukawa, 2008; Fagan & Iglesias, 1999). For example, one study found that fathers’ supportiveness when children were 2-years-old was associated with children’s intellectual functioning scores at 2- and 3-years-old (Cabrera, Shannon, & Tamis-LeMonda, 2007). Another study found that paternal warmth when children were 12-years-old was a predictor of school achievement two years later; this remained true after controlling for the effect of maternal warmth (Chen, Liu, & Li, 2000).

In addition to examining fathers and children’s cognitive func- tioning, the association between fathers’ presence in the home and children’s behavioural functioning has been considered (for re- view, see Allen & Daly, 2002). In general, research has indicated that children who experience fathers’ absence from the home at various points during childhood are more likely than other children to display internalizing problems, such as sadness, social with- drawal, and anxiety, as well as externalizing problems, such as aggression, impulsivity, and hyperactivity (Amato & Gilbreth, 1999; Carlson, 2006; Demuth & Brown, 2004). For example, one study that was conducted in the United States found that children with absent fathers displayed more antisocial behaviour than chil- dren whose fathers were present in their home, even after control- ling for the effects of paternal antisocial behaviour, SES, and presence of stepfathers (Pfiffner, McBurnett, & Rathouz, 2001).

Additional research exists illustrating the relation between pa- ternal parenting and behaviour problems in offspring (Carlson, 2006; Chen et al., 2000; Flouri, 2007; Griffin, Botvin, Scheier, Diaz, & Miller, 2000; Levine Coley, 2003). Notably, research has indicated that fathers’ use of parental control accounts for some variance in predicting child outcomes such as aggression (Chen et al., 2000). Studies have illustrated that parental control can result in both appropriate behaviour (Maccoby & Martin, 1983) as well as defiant behaviour (Baumrind, 1971) in children; the direction of the outcome most likely depends on the nature of the control employed by parents, as restrictive and authoritarian forms of control have been found to result in more negative outcomes than nonrestrictive and authoritative forms of control (Becker, 1964; Pettit, Laird, Dodge, Bates, & Criss, 2001).

Whereas the previously described studies all indicate that fa- thers’ presence in their children’s homes and parenting predict child cognitive and behavioural outcomes, some other studies have found that this is not the case after controlling for socioeconomic factors (Carlson & Corcoran, 2001; Crockett, Eggebeen, & Hawkins, 1993; DeBell, 2008; Entwisle & Alexander, 1996). Fathers’ presence is intimately connected to family SES, as fam- ilies with two working parents generally have higher incomes than single-income families. Studies also indicate that higher SES fam- ilies include fathers who display more positive parenting (Cabrera et al., 2007; Pleck, 1997). Additional studies illustrate a direct pathway between SES and child cognitive and behavioural out- comes (Cabrera et al., 2007). Socioeconomic indicators must be

considered when examining outcomes related to fathers’ presence and parenting.

Additional factors that are often considered when examining the relation between fathers and child outcomes include the quality of the home environment and the occurrence of conflict within the couple relationship. Regarding the former, previous research has demonstrated that living in a chaotic and unstimulating home environment is related to children’s cognitive functioning and behaviour problems at school (Bradley & Rock, 1985; Carlson & Corcoran, 2001; Hetherington, 1989). Regarding the latter, studies indicate that children are more likely to develop difficulties in- cluding depression, anxiety, social withdrawal, and school absence if they are regularly exposed to displays of aggression between their parents (Flouri, 2007; Grych & Fincham, 2001). Measuring the quality of the home environment typically involves the mea- surement of SES, parental absence, and parenting quality, and couple conflict has been shown to be associated with each of these factors (Fergusson & Horwood, 1998; Kaczynski, Lindahl, Malik, & Laurenceau, 2006; Sturge-Apple, Davies, & Cummings, 2006). Therefore, the home environment and couple conflict are both important to control for when predicting child developmental outcomes.

Finally, a factor that is often considered when conducting re- search in this area is child gender, as research indicates a moder- ating effect of gender in the relation between fathers and offspring development. Specifically, some studies demonstrate that fathers predict development in sons more than daughters because they serve as male role models for their sons (Biller & Kampton, 1997; Bronte-Tinkew et al., 2008; Mott, Kowaleski-Jones, & Menaghan, 1997). However, a smaller body of research has illustrated that fathers are more important for their daughters’ development than that of their sons (Levine Coley, 1998), particularly regarding nonverbal cognitive abilities (Hetherington, Camara, & Featherman, 1983; Sandqvist, 1995). As different and sometimes opposing effects have been illustrated, it is unclear how child gender moderates the association between fathers’ presence and children’s outcomes.

Many existing studies regarding the association between fathers and their children’s development have methodological limitations. For example, mothers who participate in studies are often asked to report on the parenting strategies of the children’s fathers (Roy & Kwon, 2007). Research has demonstrated, however, that reports of paternal parenting differ between mothers and fathers (Mikelson, 2008). Furthermore, most studies have not included participants who live in a cultural context in which English is not the primary language spoken and the majority of neighborhoods are socioeco- nomically disadvantaged (Roy & Kwon, 2007); notable exceptions include the Supporting Father Involvement Project (Cowan, Cowan, Pruett, & Wong, 2009) and the Fragile Families studies (Carlson & McLanahan, 2002). Moreover, the majority of studies in this area measure fathers’ presence and parenting and child development concurrently; few prospective studies exist that ex- amine the roles that fathers play on the trajectory of cognitive and behavioural development over the course of childhood (Roy & Kwon, 2007). Finally, many of the prospective studies are de- signed to examine the relation between fathers’ presence and parenting and the development of offspring between the ages of approximately 2- and 5-years-old; more research is required to

174 POUGNET, SERBIN, STACK, AND SCHWARTZMAN

understand the ways in which fathers influence the development of children in middle childhood and adolescence.

Current Study

The current study addressed some of these limitations by exam- ining the prospective associations between fathers’ presence in their children’s homes and their parenting, and cognitive and behavioural outcomes in socioeconomically at-risk children be- yond the preschool years. The study was carried out using data from the Concordia Longitudinal Risk Project (Schwartzman, Ledingham, & Serbin, 1985), an intergenerational study of low- income Francophone families in Québec (Statistics Canada, 2007b).

Based on the existing literature regarding the relation between fathers’ presence and parenting and children’s cognitive and be- havioural outcomes, it was hypothesised that (1) children who live with their fathers in middle childhood and whose fathers demon- strated positive early parenting abilities would have increased levels of cognitive functioning and (2) lower levels of behaviour problems in preadolescence than other children. It was also antic- ipated that (3) gender would moderate the relations between fa- thers’ early parenting and presence in middle childhood and cog- nitive functioning and behaviour problems later on in preadolescence. As the literature is mixed regarding the direction of the moderation, this hypothesis is exploratory.

Method

Participants

The current sample included 138 children and their families who were participants in the Concordia Longitudinal Risk Project (Schwartzman et al., 1985), a large intergenerational longitudinal research program. Participating families had been previously as- sessed when their children were in early childhood (2- to 5-years- old), when they had indicated an interest and a willingness to participate in further studies. At Time 1 (T1) of the present study, which occurred in the early 2000s, the children were between 6- and 10-years-old (M � 7.69, SD � 1.01). Seventy-six of the children were girls and 62 were boys. The families in the study had a median annual family income of $41,860 (range � $6,905.31– $145,600.00; SD � $24,918.68), and 73% of families fell below $60,118, which was the mean income level in Québec at the time (Institut de la Statistique Québec, 2009). Mothers and fathers had completed an average of 12.23 and 11.77 years of education, respectively (SD � 2.37 and 2.14, respectively), and 17% and 14% of the families had mothers and fathers who had not completed high school, respectively. At T1, 24.6% of the children (34 of 138) did not live with their fathers. This is comparable to the 21% rate of single motherhood for children of all ages in the general population of Québec in 2001 (Statistics Canada, 2003). Moreover, 54 (52%) of the fathers who lived with their children were married to the children’s mothers, 46 (45%) of the fathers were cohabiting with the children’s mothers, and 3 (3%) were separated from the children’s mothers (i.e., custodial fathers); datum regarding civic status was missing for one participant.

The second wave of data collection, or Time 2 (T2), occurred in the mid 2000s between 3 and 5 years after T1 when the children

were between 9- and 13-years-old (M � 10.06, SD � 1.56). Ninety-six of the 138 families from T1 participated at T2. The families in the study had a median annual income at T2 of $46,826 (range � $7,926.10–$178,573.20; SD � $33.295.55), and 70% of families fell below the mean income level in Québec, which at the time was $68,452 (Institut de la Statistique Québec, 2009). The mean income at T2 of $53,394.58 was not significantly greater than the mean income at T1 of $44,498.88 (t86 � �1.88, ns). When the eight families with annual T2 incomes of higher than $100,000 were treated as outliers and excluded from analyses, the results were not significantly different from those of the full sample; therefore, these families were included in the current analyses. At T2, 33.7% of the children (32 of 96) did not live with their fathers, which was not significantly different than the 24.6% rate of fathers’ absence at T1 (�2 � .02, ns). Forty-three (57%) fathers who lived with their children were married to the children’s mothers, 28 (37%) were cohabiting with the children’s mothers, and 4 (6%) were separated from the children’s mothers (i.e., custodial fathers).

Missing Data

As noted above, there was participant attrition between the two time points as well as missing data within the variable set for the 138 participants in the current study. It was hypothesised that these data were not missing completely at random, as families with lower occupational prestige, more children, and parental absence frequently have chaotic home environments and less time and motivation to provide complete data. The mean level of parental occupational prestige at T2 was significantly greater than the mean prestige level at T1 (t132 � �3.05, p � .01), and the mean number of children at T2 was significantly smaller than the mean number of children at T1 (t135 � �2.30, p � .05), indicating that those families with greater levels of occupational prestige and fewer children were more likely to remain in the study between the two time points. Results from Little’s MCAR test confirmed that data were not missing completely at random (�2 � 553.54, p � .05); therefore, multiple imputation (MI) was employed in the current study in order to estimate missing data for those variables with less than 20% of its cases missing (Allison, 2001; McKnight, McKnight, Sidani, & Figueredo, 2007). The Amelia program (Honaker, King, & Blackwell, 2006), set at a tolerance of .001, was used to impute 20 data sets (number of imputations ranged from 10 to 28). The imputed data were aggregated and used in all subsequent analyses.

Measures

Family demographics and fathers’ presence in the home. Annual family income, maternal educational attainment, paternal educational attainment, and child age were determined by partic- ipants’ responses to demographics questionnaires administered at T1 and T2. Whether or not biological fathers lived with their children was determined by asking: “With whom does your child live?” Responses were coded as 1 when biological fathers were reported as living with their children and 0 when children lived only with their mothers or other adults and siblings. The current analyses focused on fathers’ presence in the home at T1 when children were in middle childhood rather than earlier when chil-

175FATHERS’ AND CHILDREN’S FUNCTIONING

dren were preschool-aged in order to achieve greater variance in fathers’ presence (i.e., to reflect the fact that some fathers left between early childhood and T1).

Parental control. The Parenting Dimensions Inventory– Short Version (PDI; Power, 2002) is a 27-item self-report measure of five dimensions of parenting that had been administered to fathers 1 to 3 years prior to the current waves of data collection, when their children were between 2- and 5-years-old. It was administered during this period because more fathers were present to participate in data collection when their children were preschool-aged than when their children were older, and it was used to capture the impact that fathering children in this sensitive period had on later development. To assess the type of parental control employed with children, the Type of Control dimension of the PDI was used for the present study. This dimension employs 11 questions with a 4-point Likert scale response format to assess the mechanisms through which parents guide and shape the behaviour of their children, and it includes Physical Punishment, Material or Social Consequences, Reasoning, Scolding, and Reminding scales. For the purposes of the current study, an average of the five scale scores was used in the analyses (� � .69, 95% Confidence Interval (CI) � .55–.79, M � 1.29, SD � .53). Higher scores indicate more positive forms of control. Alpha values for the scales that make up the Type of Control dimension have been found to range from .84 to .92 and have good validity (Power, 2002; Slater & Power, 1987).

Couple conflict. Prior to the current waves of data collection when children were preschool-aged, mothers and fathers in the sample completed the Conflict Tactics Scale (Straus, 1979), a self-report measure of intimate partner and child maltreatment. Scales measure sexual, psychological, and physical attacks com- mitted by both partners over the past 12 months as well as anytime in the past. Nine items that inquire about lifetime occurrences of couple conflict (e.g., Have you ever thrown anything at your partner, or has your partner ever thrown anything at you?) were included in the current data analyses (� � .84, 95% CI � .82–.86, M � .11, SD � .01). Item responses were coded as 1 when couple conflict was reported and 0 when couple conflict was not reported. The complete measure has good reliability, indicated by an alpha of .88 for couple conflict, as well as good concurrent and construct validity (Straus, 1979).

Home environment. The quality of the families’ home en- vironment was assessed at T2 using the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment–Revised Edition scale (HOME; Caldwell & Bradley, 1984). This instrument combines observa- tions by trained researchers with structured interviews with both parents (if available) to obtain information about children’s home environment. This 59-item tool is broken down into eight scales that assess parental responsivity, physical environment, learning materials, active stimulation, encouragement of maturity, emo- tional climate, parental involvement, and family participation; for the purposes of the current study, an overall score was used in the analyses (� � .61, CI � .48–.71, M � 40.66, SD � 5.35). Scores range from 0 to 59, and higher scores indicate higher quality home environments. The HOME inventory is a well-standardized mea- sure that demonstrates good reliability and validity properties (Totsika & Sylva, 2004).

Cognitive functioning. Children’s cognitive functioning was assessed at two different times. Three years prior to the two waves

of data collection described in the current study, the Bayley Scales of Infant Development–Second Edition (Bayley, 1993) had been administered to those children who were under 42-months-old (n � 66), and the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale–Fourth Edition (Thorndike, Hagen, & Sattler, 1987) had been administered to those children between 42- and 72-months-old (n � 72). Each child had received an IQ score that was converted into a z-score and then employed in the analyses in order to control for the overall stability of IQ over time.

At T2, the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Third Edi- tion (WISC–III: Wechsler, 1991) was administered. This com- monly used measure of children’s intellectual functioning provides Verbal IQ scores, which are an indication of children’s skills in language and comprehension, as well as Performance IQ scores, which are an indication of children’s visual-spatial and nonverbal analytical reasoning skills. The WISC–III was administered to children when they were between the ages of 9- and 13-years-old by individuals with master’s level training or above in clinical psychology. Children’s Verbal IQs ranged from 62 to 133 (M � 98.59, SD � 15.82), and their Performance IQs ranged from 72 to 140 (M � 101.86, SD � 11.87).

Behaviour problems. Teachers assessed children’s behav- iour problems at school by completing the Child Behaviour Checklist–Teacher Report Form (TRF; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) at T2 when children were between 9- and 13-years-old. This commonly used measure of behavioural functioning includes 120 items for which the respondent assigns a score between 0 and 2 for each question; a score of 0 indicates an absence of the behaviour, a score of 1 indicates that the child performs the behaviour some- times, and a score of 2 indicates that the child often performs the behaviour. Scores of externalizing and internalizing behaviours are obtained, as well as an overall problem behaviour score that encompasses both internalizing and externalizing scores. Higher scores indicate increased problem behaviours. Teachers were em- ployed as informants of children’s behaviour problems rather than mothers, fathers, or the children because fathers’ absence can result in parent conflict, maternal distress, and child distress. Teachers were assumed to be a somewhat more independent source of information than mothers, fathers, or children themselves when examining the effects of fathers’ absence on offspring be- haviour problems. The children in the full sample ranged in TRF externalizing behaviour T scores from 39 to 78 (M � 53.19, SD � 8.10), and they ranged in TRF internalizing behaviour T scores from 37 to 76 (M � 54.88, SD � 8.30). Twenty-eight percent of the children had TRF problem T scores in the “borderline” range or above (T score 60), compared with an expected 18% of nonre- ferred children in the general population (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001).

Procedure

Families participated in the current study at two different time points (T1 and T2) that were 3 to 5 years apart. At each time point in the current study, the children’s families were contacted by telephone to solicit participation. Those who agreed at T1 were mailed a package of questionnaires including a demographics measure assessing family structure. At T2, families were again contacted and consent was obtained over the telephone and sub- sequently in writing. Children whose families agreed to participate

176 POUGNET, SERBIN, STACK, AND SCHWARTZMAN

were tested both at school and at home, in three separate sessions spanning over a 2-week period. Demographics questionnaires were completed, and information regarding family structure, maternal educational attainment, annual family income, and children’s age was obtained. Also at T2, the HOME inventory was administered to families. During this wave of data collection, children under- went cognitive testing, and teachers were asked to complete the TRF for the target children. Families were financially compensated for their participation in the first and second waves of the study.

Strategy for Analysis

Hierarchical linear regression analyses were performed with the full sample in predicting children’s T2 IQ and TRF scores. Key assumptions of linear regression, including linear relationships, little multicollinearity, and no auto-correlation were tested, and the number of predictors used in each analysis was appropriate for the number of participants in the study. As 138 families participated in the current study, there was power to detect a small to moderate effect or greater; effect sizes as small as .06 were found to be statistically significant in the current analyses.

In the first model of the analyses, children’s gender and age at T2 were entered into the equation. Family income, maternal edu- cation, and paternal education at T2 were entered into the second model in order to account for socioeconomic factors. Subse- quently, fathers’ presence at T1, the quality of the home environ- ment at T2, early couple conflict, fathers’ use of early control, and early IQ were entered respectively in separate models of the analyses. Additional predictors (e.g., parents’ relationship status, mothers’ use of early control) were included in preliminary anal- yses; however, because the number of participants allowed for a maximum number of predictors, those predictors that were not statistically significantly associated with the outcome variables were excluded from the final regression analyses. Interactions between fathers’ presence at T1 as well as fathers’ use of early control and each of the other predictors were examined in order to ascertain any possible moderation effects by entering the interac- tion terms in the final models of each of the regression analyses.

Lastly, median splits were conducted with the continuous predic- tors in order to graphically depict statistically significant and interesting interactions.

Results

The first hypothesis held that children who live with their fathers in middle childhood and whose fathers demonstrated pos- itive early parental control would have higher levels of cognitive functioning later on in development than other children. The point biserial correlation between T1 fathers’ presence and T2 Perfor- mance IQ was statistically significant, r(136) � .18, p � .05, as was the Pearson correlation between fathers’ use of positive con- trol and T2 Performance IQ, r(136) � .35, p � .01. Preliminary analyses indicated that Verbal IQ was not associated with either fathers’ presence, r(136) � .05, ns, or fathers’ control, r(136) � .19, ns (see Table 1); therefore, the following results describe only analyses with Performance IQ.

Hierarchical linear regression was employed to predict T2 Per- formance IQ. In the final model of the main effects analysis, F(10, 137) � 9.99, p � .01, there was a statistically significant effect for fathers’ use of control (� � .38, p � .01), although the effect of fathers’ presence was not statistically significant (� � .13, ns; see Table 2). Thus, the first hypothesis was supported for paternal control but not for fathers’ presence. Statistically significant main effects for T2 home environment, couple conflict, and early IQ were also found.

Interactions between the predictors of T2 Performance IQ were subsequently examined. The interaction between T1 fathers’ pres- ence and T2 paternal education predicted T2 Performance IQ when it was entered into the regression analysis, such that for those children whose fathers had fewer years of education, having a father absent predicted lower Performance IQ scores than having a father present (B � �4.53, p � .01; see Figure 1).

In summary, children whose fathers displayed more positive early parental control had higher Performance IQ scores at T2 than other children after controlling for family and socioeconomic factors. In addition, fathers’ presence at T1 predicted higher Per-

Table 1 Correlations Between All Variables in the Full Sample (n � 138)

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14

1. T1 father presencepb — �.09 .18� .07 �.14t �.15t .09 �.11 .34�� .20� .11 .15t .38�� �.23��

2. Father control — .35�� .19t �.45�� .02 .26�� �.04 �.06 �.37�� .07 .05 .02 .13 3. T2 Performance IQ — .59�� �.35�� �.07 .20� �.02 .20� .19� .29�� .50�� .19� �.24��

4. T2 Verbal IQ — �.29�� �.08 .01 �.16 .27�� .41�� .19t .54�� .29�� �.32��

5. T2 TRF internalizing — .26�� �.27�� .05 .07 �.01 .02 �.22� �.24�� .18�

6. T2 TRF externalizing — �.30�� .32�� �.11 �.17� �.19� �.24�� �.53�� .18�

7. Child genderpb — �.10 �.03 .00 �.06 .21� .16t .08 8. T2 child age — �.14t �.13 �.06 .00 �.22� .16t

9. T2 income — .58�� .24�� .20� .52�� �.27��

10. T2 maternal education

— .40�� .41�� .47�� �.38��

11. T2 paternal education — .26�� .15t �.17�

12. Early IQ — .39�� �.26��

13. T2 HOME — �.46��

14. Couple conflictpb —

Note. T1 � Time 1; T2 � Time 2; pb � point biserial correlations; TRF � Teacher Report Form; HOME � Home Environment. t p � .10. � p � .05. �� p � .01.

177FATHERS’ AND CHILDREN’S FUNCTIONING

formance IQ scores at T2 for fathers with fewer years of educa- tional attainment.

The second hypothesis held that children who live with their fathers in middle childhood and whose fathers demonstrated pos- itive early parenting skills would have lower levels of behaviour problems later on in development than other children. Preliminary analysis indicated that the point biserial correlation between T2 TRF externalizing and T1 fathers’ presence, r(136) � �.15, ns, as well as the Pearson correlation between T2 TRF externalizing and early paternal control, r(136) � .02, ns, were not statistically significant. The Pearson correlation between T2 TRF internalizing and fathers’ early use of control was statistically significant, r(136) � �.45, p � .01, although the point biserial correlation between T1 fathers’ presence and T2 TRF internalizing was not statistically significant, r(136) � �.14, ns (see Table 1). Conse-

quently, the following results describe only results with T2 TRF internalizing problems.

Hierarchical linear regression was used to predict T2 internal- izing problems. In the final model of the main effects analysis, F(10, 137) � 8.55, p � .01, there were statistically significant main effects for early fathers’ control (� � �.58, p � .01) and T1 fathers’ presence (� � �.18, p � .05) in predicting T2 internal- izing problems, supporting the second hypothesis. There were also statistically significant main effects for T2 income, maternal and paternal education, and early couple conflict (see Table 3).

Interactions between the predictors of T2 internalizing problems were subsequently examined. The interaction between children’s gender and T1 fathers’ presence predicted TRF internalizing prob- lems, such that girls with fathers who were present had lower internalizing problem behaviour scores than girls with fathers who were absent (B � �5.21, p � .05; see Figure 2), while the same was not true for boys.

Taken together, children whose fathers displayed less positive parental control and whose fathers were absent had higher inter- nalizing scores later on than other children. Further, fathers’ pres- ence predicted decreased internalizing problem scores later on for girls but not for boys.

Discussion

The aim of the current study was to illustrate the ways in which fathers’ presence in the home and parenting predict developmental outcomes of children over time in a disadvantaged sample living within a Québec context. Overall, the results indicated that fathers’ early use of control and fathers’ presence in middle childhood predicted cognitive and behavioural outcomes later on in develop- ment. This was illustrated utilizing a prospective methodology with a culturally and linguistically distinct longitudinal sample of socioeconomically at-risk families, and after accounting for such

Table 2 Hierarchical Linear Regressions for T2 Performance IQ in the Full Sample (n � 138)

Model

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Interactions

� � � � � � � Part B

Child gender .20� .22�� .21�� .21� .24�� .13 .06 .06 Child age (T2) �.00 .04 .04 .05 .06 .08 .03 .03 Family income (T2) .15 .11 .10 .13 .03 .10 .07 Maternal education (T2) �.01 �.01 �.02 �.07 .23t .07 .04 Paternal education (T2) .28�� .27�� .27�� .27�� .14t .10 .08 Father presence (T1) .10 .09 .07 .14t .13t .12 HOME (T2) .04 �.04 �.13 �.22� �.16 Early couple conflict �.22� �.23�� �.19� �.16 Early father control .44�� .38�� .31 Early IQ .41�� .34 Father presence �

paternal education �4.53��

Father control � gender �12.48t

F 2.74t 4.73�� 4.17�� 3.57�� 3.91�� 6.78�� 9.99��

F�change 2.74t 5.86�� 1.30 .16 5.39� 24.19�� 26.63��

R2 (total adjusted) .03 .12 .12 .12 .15 .28 .40

Note. T1 � Time 1; T2 � Time 2; HOME � Home Environment. B represents the unstandardized b coefficient (the effect of an independent variable on the dependent variable, net of the effects of the other independent variables). t p � .10. � p � .05. �� p � .01.

Figure 1. Interaction between fathers’ presence in middle childhood and paternal educational attainment in preadolescence in predicting later Per- formance IQ scores in preadolescence.

178 POUGNET, SERBIN, STACK, AND SCHWARTZMAN

potentially confounding factors as annual family income, the qual- ity of the home environment, parental educational attainment, and couple conflict.

The first hypothesis that children whose biological fathers dem- onstrated positive early parental control and were present in mid- dle childhood would have increased levels of cognitive functioning later on than other children was partially supported, although only for nonverbal cognitive functioning. Specifically, children whose fathers used more positive early parental control had higher Per- formance IQ scores later in development than other children. Past research suggests that fathers enhance their children’s cognitive functioning through play (MacDonald & Parke, 1986; Pruett, 1998); perhaps fathers who demonstrated the positive use of pa-

rental control during play strengthened children’s nonverbal abil- ities. The effect of paternal control on children’s nonverbal cog- nitive functioning was statistically significant over a span of 6 to 10 years (i.e., early childhood to preadolescence) and accounted for more variance in Performance IQ scores than any other pre- dictor, even when “baseline” IQ at preschool age was controlled.

In addition to nonverbal cognitive functioning, results from the current study demonstrated that children whose fathers displayed more positive early control and were present in middle childhood had fewer behaviour problems at school later in development compared with other children, supporting the second hypothesis. However, this was only true for internalizing behaviour problems, a result that contradicts studies that suggest fathers’ involvement predicts both internalizing and externalizing problems in children (Carlson, 2006; Chen et al., 2000). On the other hand, a study by Levine Coley (2003) that examined African American father- daughter relationships indicated that daughters whose fathers were absent and who experienced alienation and disengagement in their relationships with their fathers were more likely to also experience symptoms of depression and problem behaviours at school but not aggressive behaviours. It is possible that the results from the current study are similarly due to the quality of preadolescent children’s relationships with their present and absent fathers. Father-daughter relationship quality was not measured in the cur- rent study, but it might be more predictive of internalizing prob- lems than externalizing problems in offspring.

In examining statistical interactions in predicting internalizing problems in preadolescence, the current results suggest that girls whose fathers were absent in middle childhood had significantly higher levels of internalizing behaviours at school than girls whose fathers were present; the same was not true for boys. Past research has illustrated significant associations between fathers’ absence and increased behaviour problems in children (Chen et al., 2000; Carlson, 2006; Demuth & Brown, 2004; Flouri, 2007; Pfiffner et al., 2001); however, this is the first study to our knowledge that has found these associations for daughters and not sons. It is possible

Table 3 Hierarchical Linear Regressions for T2 Teacher Report Form Internalizing Problems in the Full Sample (n � 138)

Model

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Interactions

� � � � � � � Part B

Child gender �.27�� �.27�� �.25�� �.23�� �.23�� �.08 �.07 �.07 Child age (T2) .02 .03 .02 �.03 �.03 �.04 �.04 �.04 Family income (T2) .11 .16 .23� .23� .36�� .35� .25 Maternal education (T2) �.07 �.07 .04 .04 �.35�� �.33�� �.20 Paternal education (T2) .01 .02 .01 .01 .18� .18� .16 Father presence (T1) �.16t �.09 �.08 �.18� �.18� �.16 HOME (T2) �.31�� �.25� �.13 �.12 �.09 Early couple conflict .15 .17� .16� .14 Early father control �.59�� �.58�� �.47 Early IQ �.04 �.04 Father presence � gender �5.21�

F 5.39�� 2.34� 2.50� 3.51�� 3.44�� 9.53�� 8.55��

F�change 5.39�� .36 3.10t 8.70�� 2.62 48.19�� .26 R2 (total adjusted) .06 .05 .06 .11 .13 .36 .36

Note. T1 � Time 1; T2 � Time 2; HOME � Home Environment. B represents the unstandardized b coefficient (the effect of an independent variable on the dependent variable, net of the effects of the other independent variables). t p � .10. � p � .05. �� p � .01.

Figure 2. Interaction between fathers’ presence in middle childhood and children’s gender in predicting later Teacher Report Form (TRF) internal- izing problems in preadolescence.

179FATHERS’ AND CHILDREN’S FUNCTIONING

that these findings are a result of girls’ increased stress and negative affect because of factors that have been found to be associated with breakdowns in marital relationships and fathers’ absence such as family discord, mothers’ difficulties upon family disruption, and negative father-daughter relationships (Kerig, Cowan, & Cowan, 1993; Sigle-Rushton & McLanahan, 2004); these factors were not measured in the current study, so this remains a hypothesis for future research. Furthermore, perhaps fathers’ presence influenced the type rather than the level of internalizing problems that boys experience at school (i.e., sadness when father is absent vs. anxiety when father is present), a differ- ence that was not captured in the current analyses. Future research with a larger sample could use the Syndrome and DSM-oriented scales of the TRF to examine whether various internalizing symp- toms are differentially related to fathers’ presence in girls and boys.

In both of the regression analyses, fathers’ early use of control enhanced the effect of fathers’ presence during middle childhood in predicting preadolescent cognitive and behavioural outcomes. Specifically, fathers’ presence marginally predicted Performance IQ and significantly predicted internalizing problems at school only when fathers’ control was included in the model. It is possible that this suppression effect is due to the nature of the measurement of paternal control in the current study, as those fathers who reported using the most positive types of early parental control might have also been the fathers who remained living with their children between three and five years later. Past research has indicated a significant association between family structure and paternal parenting (e.g., Carlson, 2006; Griffin et al., 2000); how- ever, fathers’ use of early control and fathers’ presence during middle childhood were not significantly correlated in the current study. Further research is required to fully understand the relation between fathers’ presence and parenting characteristics in predict- ing outcomes in offspring.

The current study illustrated the significant effects of socioeco- nomic factors on children’s concurrent cognitive functioning and behaviour problems. Specifically, the quality of the home envi- ronment predicted children’s concurrent Performance IQ. Because the regression coefficient was negative, a suppressor effect might have occurred; this result should be replicated by future research before making further interpretations. Furthermore, family income, maternal education, and paternal education predicted children’s concurrent internalizing problems. Unexpectedly, fathers who were more educated had children with higher levels of internaliz- ing problems; this finding had no precendent in the literature and should await replication before interpreting it. In general, fathers’ presence and early control predicted children’s later development even after accounting for SES factors, supporting results from previous studies (Cabrera et al., 2007; Ryan et al., 2006). These results suggest that fathers’ presence in middle childhood and early control might be important for children’s later cognitive and be- havioural functioning for reasons other than fathers’ income con- tribution to the family, even among socioeconomically at-risk families.

Although the present study contributed to the literature in a number of ways, it also had several limitations. The size of the sample was small; this limited the number of control variables that could be included in the regression analyses. An additional limi- tation is the use of fathers’ presence as a predictor despite the

argument that it is not detailed enough to understand children’s experiences (Flouri, 2007), and more specifically, the lack of differentiation between fathers’ absence from their children’s homes and fathers’ absence from their children’s lives. It is likely that some children in the sample who did not live with their biological fathers had regular contact with their fathers, which could perhaps account for the small and marginal associations between fathers’ presence and child outcomes in the current study. Had father involvement been measured in addition to fathers’ presence, it might have mediated the relation between fathers’ absence and children’s outcomes, as has been demonstrated in some previous research (e.g., Astone & McLanahan, 1991; Allen & Daly, 2002; Carlson, 2006).

Moreover, the measurement of fathers’ parenting was restricted to parental control used with young children; an interesting line of future inquiry would involve examining additional parenting di- mensions in the interactions between fathers and children later in development, a direction that could not be pursued in the current study due to lack of additional parenting measures in the original data set. Additionally, measurements of fathers’ parenting were obtained by self-report questionnaires; richer information regard- ing father-child relationships could be obtained by observing fa- thers in interactions with their children. Moreover, some scales that were employed in the current study demonstrated low Cronbach’s alpha coefficients (e.g., PDI, HOME); therefore, the correlations that were found might have been attenuated due to greater measurement error. Finally, only teachers’ reports of chil- dren’s behaviour problems were included in the current analyses. As previous research has indicated that reports of the development of behaviour problems vary within and across informants (Keiley, Bates, Dodge, & Pettit, 2000), future analyses could consider reports from parents and the children themselves in addition to teachers’ reports to gain a comprehensive view of children’s behaviour problems.

In conclusion, fathers’ early use of positive control predicted both increased nonverbal cognitive performance and decreased internalizing problems later in development, and fathers’ presence in middle childhood predicted decreased internalizing problems later in development. The question of family composition is espe- cially relevant in Québec, as the rate of common-law unions is much higher than in other Canadian provinces (25% of families with children in Québec compared with 5.5% in Canada; Statistics Canada, 2007b). Further, common-law unions are more easily and more often dissolved than marriages (Andersson, 2002; Kamp Dush & Amato, 2005), frequently resulting in single-mother fam- ilies. Because of Québec’s unique demographics and social cus- toms, the current research suggests that it is essential that the province formulate policies that would encourage increased and positive forms of contact between children and their fathers. Ini- tiatives such as parental leave for men and parenting classes that emphasise the role of fathers could help to maximize children’s development from early childhood to preadolescence.

Résumé

Une littérature émergente illustre l’influence des pères dans le développement de leurs enfants. Ce sujet est particulièrement pertinent au Québec, une province unique sur les plan culturel et démographique, où la monoparentalité féminine est relativement

180 POUGNET, SERBIN, STACK, AND SCHWARTZMAN

commune; cette situation est associée à des désavantages so- cioéconomiques prédisant des effets cognitifs et comportementaux adverses chez les jeunes. À l’aide des données du Concordia longitudinal risk project, une banque de données intergénéra- tionnelle recueillies dans la région de Montréal, la présente étude visait à examiner les relations prospectives entre la présence et la parentalité du père et le fonctionnement cognitif et comportemen- tal subséquent de l’enfant. L’échantillon actuel comptait 138 fa- milles de classe économique faible à moyenne qui ont participé à deux vagues de collecte de données : lorsque les enfants étaient d’âge moyen et trois ou cinq ans après, lorsqu’ils étaient à la préadolescence. Les résultats ont indiqué que pour les filles seule- ment, la présence du père en enfance prédisait moins de problèmes internalisés à la préadolescence. Pour les garçons et les filles, le contrôle parental positif du père prédisait un meilleur QI de per- formance et moins de problèmes internalisés jusqu’à six ans plus tard. Ces résultats ajoutent à la littérature croissante suggérant que les pères contribuent de façon importante au fonctionnement cog- nitif et comportemental de leurs enfants et appuient les politiques visant à encourager les pères à passer du temps avec leurs enfants (c.-à-d., congés parentaux pour les hommes) et qui promeuvent un rôle et une implication positives du père à travers des cours de parentalité.

Mots-clés : pères, problèmes comportementaux, fonctionnement cognitif, contexte canadien, risque socioéconomique

References

Achenbach, T., & Rescorla, L. (2001). Manual for ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.

Allen, S., & Daly, K. (2002). The effects of father involvement: A summary of the research evidence. Carleton Place, Ontario, Canada: Father In- volvement Initiative–Ontario Network.

Allison, P. (2001). Missing data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Amato, P., & Gilbreth, J. (1999). Nonresident fathers and children’s

well-being: A meta-analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61(3), 557–573.

Andersson, G. (2002). Children’s experience of family disruption and family formation: Evidence from 16 FFS countries. Demographic Re- search, 7, 343–364.

Astone, N., & McLanahan, S. (1991). Family structure, parental practices, and high school completion. American Sociological Review, 56, 309– 320.

Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmen- tal Psychology Monograph, 4(1), 1–103.

Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley scales of infant development (2nd ed.). New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation.

Becker, W. (1964). Consequences of different kinds of parental discipline. In M. Hoffman & L. Hoffman (Eds.), Review of child development research (pp. 169–208). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

Biller, H., & Kampton, J. (1997). The father and the school-aged child. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (pp. 143–163). New York, NY: Wiley Interscience.

Bradley, R., & Rock, S. (1985). The HOME inventory: Its relation to school failure and development of an elementary-age version. In W. Frankenburg, R. Emde, & J. Sullivan (Eds.), Early identification of children at risk: An international perspective (pp. 159–173). New York, NY: Plenum Press Publishing Corporation.

Bronte-Tinkew, J., Carrano, J., Horowitz, A., & Kinukawa, A. (2008).

Involvement among resident fathers and links to infant cognitive out- comes. Journal of Family Issues, 29(9), 1211–1244.

Cabrera, N., Shannon, J., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. (2007). Fathers’ influence on their children’s cognitive and emotional development: From toddlers to pre-k. Applied Development Science, 11(4), 208–213.

Caldwell, B., & Bradley, R. (1984). Manual for the home observation for measurement of the environment (Revised ed.). Little Rock, AR: Uni- versity of Arkansas.

Carlson, M. (2006). Family structure, father involvement, and adolescent behavioral outcomes. Journal of Marriage and Family, 68, 137–154.

Carlson, M., & Corcoran, M. (2001). Family structure and children’s behavioral and cognitive outcomes. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 779–792.

Carlson, M., & McLanahan, S. (2002). Father involvement, fragile fami- lies, and public policy. In C. Tamis-LeMonda & N. Cabrera (Eds.), Handbook of father involvement: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 461–488). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Chen, X., Liu, M., & Li., D. (2000). Parental warmth, control, and indul- gence and their relations to adjustment in Chinese children: A longitu- dinal study. Journal of Family Psychology, 14(3), 401–419.

Cowan, P., Cowan, C., Pruett, M., & Wong, J. (2009). Promoting fathers’ engagement with children: Preventive interventions for low-income families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 71, 663–679.

Crockett, L., Eggebeen, D., & Hawkins, A. (1993). Father’s presence and young children’s behavioral and cognitive adjustment. Journal of Fam- ily Issues, 14(3), 355–377.

DeBell, M. (2008). Children living without their fathers: Population esti- mates and indicators of educational well-being. Social Indicators Re- search, 87, 427–443.

Demuth, S., & Brown, S. (2004). Family structure, family processes, and adolescent delinquency: The significance of parental absence versus parental gender. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 41, 58–81.

Entwisle, D., & Alexander, K. (1996). Family type and children’s growth in reading and math over the primary grades. Journal of Marriage and Family, 58, 341–355.

Fagan, J., & Iglesias, A. (1999). Father involvement program effects on fathers, father figures, and their Head Start children: A quasi- experimental study. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 14(2), 243– 269.

Fergusson, D., & Horwood, L. (1998). Exposure to interparental violence in childhood and psychosocial adjustment in young adulthood. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22(5), 339–357.

Flouri, E. (2007). Fathering and adolescents’ psychological adjustment: The role of fathers’ involvement, residence and biology status. Child: Care, Health and Development, 34(2), 152–161.

Gouvernement du Québec. (2008). Indicateurs de l’éducation, édition 2008. Québec, Canada: Ministère de l’Éducation, du Loisir et du Sport.

Griffin, K., Botvin, G., Scheier, L., Diaz, T., & Miller, N. (2000). Parenting practices as predictors of substance use, delinquency, and aggression among urban minority youth : Moderating effects of family structure and gender. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 14(2), 174–184.

Grych, J., & Fincham, F. (2001). Interparental conflict and child develop- ment: Theory, research, and applications. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

Hetherington, E. (1989). Coping with family transitions: Winners, losers, and survivors. Child Development, 60, 1–14.

Hetherington, E., Camara, K., & Featherman, D. (1983). Achievement and intellectual functioning of children in one-parent households. In J. T. Spence (Ed.), Achievement and achievement motives (pp. 205–284). San Francisco, CA: Freeman.

Honaker, J., King, G., & Blackwell, M. (2006). Amelia software web site. Retrieved from http://gking.harvard.edu/amelia

Institut de la Statistique Québec. (2009). Revenu moyen des unités famil-

181FATHERS’ AND CHILDREN’S FUNCTIONING

iales selon le type de revenu, Québec, 1996 à 2007 (table). Retrieved from http://www.stat.gouv.qc.ca/donstat/societe/famls_mengs_niv_vie/ revenus_depense/revenus/revfam96_2007.htm

Kaczynski, K., Lindahl, K., Malik, N., & Laurenceau, P. (2006). Marital conflict, maternal and paternal parenting, and child adjustment: A test of mediation and moderation. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 199–208.

Kamp Dush, C., & Amato, P. (2005). Consequences of relationship status and quality for subjective well-being. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22(5), 607–627.

Keiley, M., Bates, J., Dodge, K., & Pettit, G. (2000). A cross-domain growth analysis: Externalizing and internalizing behaviors during 8 years of childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28(2), 161–179.

Kerig, P., Cowan, P., & Cowan, C. (1993). Marital quality and gender differences in parent-child interaction. Developmental Psychology, 29(6), 931–939.

Levine Coley, R. (1998). Children’s socialization experiences and func- tioning in single-mother households: The importance of fathers and other men. Child Development, 9(1), 219–230.

Levine Coley, R. (2003). Daughter-father relationships and adolescent psychosocial functioning in low-income African American families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 65, 867–875.

Maccoby, E., & Martin, J. (1983). Socialization in the context of family: Parent-child interaction. In E. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Socialization, personality and social development (pp. 1–101). New York, NY: Wiley Interscience.

MacDonald, K., & Parke, R. (1986). Parent-child physical play: The effects of sex and age of children and parents. Sex Roles, 15, 367–378.

McKnight, P., McKnight, K., Sidani, S., & Figueredo, A. (2007). Missing data: A gentle introduction. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Mikelson, K. (2008). He said, she said: Comparing mother and father reports of father involvement. Journal of Marriage and Family, 70(3), 613–624.

Mott, F., Kowaleski-Jones, L., & Menaghan, E. (1997). Paternal absence and child behavior: Does a child’s gender make a difference? Journal of Marriage and the Family, 59, 103–118.

Pettit, G., Laird, R., Dodge, K., Bates, J., & Criss, M. (2001). Antecedents and behavior-problem outcomes of parental monitoring and psycholog- ical control in early adolescence. Child Development, 72(2), 583–598.

Pfiffner, L., McBurnett, K., & Rathouz, P. (2001). Father absence and familial antisocial characteristics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 29(5), 357–367.

Pleck, J. (1997). Paternal involvement: Levels, sources, and consequences. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (pp. 66–103). New York, NY: Wiley.

Power, T. (2002). Parenting dimensions inventory (PDI): A research manual. Unpublished document, University of Houston, Houston, TX.

Pruett, K. (1998). Role of the father. Pediatrics, 102(5S), 1253–1261. Ricciuti, H. (2004). Single parenthood, achievement, and problem behavior

in white, black, and Hispanic children. Journal of Education Research, 97(4), 196–206.

Roy, K., & Kwon, Y. (2007). Qualitative insights and methodological challenges: Next steps in research on low-income fathering. Applied Development Science, 11(4), 234–238.

Ryan, R., Martin, A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2006). Is one good parent good

enough? Patterns of mother and father parenting and child cognitive outcomes at 24 and 36 months. Parenting: Science and Practice, 6(2), 211–228.

Sandqvist, K. (1995). Verbal boys and mathematical girls – Family back- ground and educational careers. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 39(1), 5–36.

Schwartzman, E., Ledingham, J., & Serbin, L. (1985). Identification of children at risk for adult schizophrenia: A longitudinal study. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Appliquée, 34(3), 363–380.

Sigle-Rushton, W., & McLanahan, S. (2004), Father absence and child wellbeing: A critical review. In D. Moynihan, L. Rainwater, and T. Smeeding (Eds.), The future of the family (pp. 116–155). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

Slater, M., & Power, T. (1987). Multidimensional assessment of parenting in single-parent families. In J. Vincent (Ed.), Advances in family inter- vention, assessment, and theory (pp. 197–228). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Statistics Canada. (2003). Familles selon la structure et le nombre d’enfants de tous ages, Canada, Québec et Ontario (table). Families: Summary tables, 2001 census. Retrieved from http://www.stat.gouv .qc.ca/donstat/societe/famls_mengs_niv_vie/menage_famille/men_ fam_enf/familles/tableau_17.htm

Statistics Canada. (2007a). Census families in private households by family structure and presence of children, by province and territory (table). Family types: Summary tables, 2006 census. Retrieved from http:// www40.statcan.gc.ca/l01/cst01/famil54a-eng.htm

Statistics Canada. (2007b). Census families by number of children at home, by province and territory (table). Marriage and common-law unions: Summary tables, 2006 census. Retrieved from http://www40 .statcan.gc.ca/l01/cst01/famil50f-eng.htm

Statistics Canada. (2009). Persons in low income after tax (table). Low income and equality: Summary tables, 2006 census (Catalogue no. 75–202-X). Retrieved from http://www40.statcan.gc.ca/l01/cst01/ famil19a-eng.htm

Straus, M. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The Con- flict Tactic (CT) Scales. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 75–88.

Sturge-Apple, M., Davies, P., & Cummings, E. (2006). Impact of hostility and withdrawal in interparental conflict on parental emotional unavail- ability and children’s adjustment difficulties. Child Development, 77(6), 1623–1641.

Teachman, J., Day, R., Paasch, K., Carver, K., & Call, V. (1998). Sibling resemblance in behavioral and cognitive outcomes: The role of father presence. Journal of Marriage and Family, 60, 835–848.

Thorndike, R., Hagen, E., & Sattler, J. (1987). The Stanford-Binet intelli- gence scale (4th ed.). Chicago, IL: Riverside.

Totsika, V., & Sylva, K. (2004). The Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment revisited. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 9(1), 25–35.

Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Received June 3, 2010 Revision received February 25, 2011

Accepted February 28, 2011 �

182 POUGNET, SERBIN, STACK, AND SCHWARTZMAN

Students with neurological problems may need special education services, adaptation of physical facilities, knowledge of special equipment, use of technology, and modification of curricular activities. Conditions such as TBI, cerebral palsy, and the like, have neurological foundations that impact the students’ speech-language skills, and ultimately, academic performance. Communication deficits may be part of another condition in which the student has motor impairments, perceptual difficulties, cognitive deficits, behavioral problems, etc. Problems may be subtle or highly noticeable.

1

2

3

4

CP – a disorder of voluntary movement and posture. No clear relationship (if any) between the degree of motor impairment and degree of intellectual impairment

5

Cerebral palsy is often associated with other disabilities. It is not unusual to find children with cerebral palsy who also have hearing or vision problems or who have intellectual impairments. We know that all these conditions can adversely affect language.

6

Children with cerebral palsy are at significant risk for speech and language difficulties. The speech difficulties of children with cerebral palsy are extremely heterogeneous and depend, to a great extent, on the specific nature of the physical disability. Individuals with cerebral palsy may have speech production difficulties in one or several areas, including respiration (e.g., rapid, shallow breathing), phonation (inadequate airflow), resonation (hypernasality), and articulation. Articulation is often affected because of difficulty controlling the tongue, lips, or mouth.

Differences in early development may have an impact on language. Studies of the interactions between children with cerebral palsy and their parents has shown a pattern of conversational dominance by parents and child passivity. Typically, parents have been reported to initiate most conversational exchanges, introduce topics, ask many closed questions, and issue many commands. Children tend to reply with limited information and fail to take a similar number of turns, to initiate an equal number of exchanges, or to use a full range of pragmatic functions. Children with cerebral palsy who have better speech intelligibility have been found to initiate more conversations and to use their communication for a wider range of functions compared with nonspeaking children.

Kuder, S. Jay. Teaching Students with Language and Communication Disabilities (The Pearson Communication Sciences and Disorders Series) (p. 191). Pearson Education. Kindle

7

Edition.

7

8

Other neuromotor disorders, such as spina bifida can also affect language acquisition and development. Spina bifida refers to a group of conditions in which a portion of the spinal cord is not completely enclosed by the vertebrae in the spinal column. In some cases, part of the spinal cord protrudes. In the most serious form of the disorder, myelomeningocele, damage to the spinal cord can cause sensory and motor losses. In addition, in about 80 percent of the cases, fluid accumulates in the brain, causing the condition known as hydrocephalus. If not treated quickly, hydrocephalus can cause Intellectual disabilities. Surgery can often correct the spinal cord abnormality in spinal bifida, but some sensory and motor disabilities can remain.

Impairments in language are characteristic of many children with spina bifida, especially those who also have hydrocephalus. Although children with spina bifida often have strengths involving vocabulary and grammar, difficulties with the use of language in context have frequently been reported. Sometimes called the “cocktail chatter” phenomenon, their communicative interactions have been described as “chatty” conversations that remain at a superficial level. In addition, although their speech may be fluent and well-articulated, it may include verbal perseveration, excessive use of stereotyped social utterances, and overfamiliarity.

Many children with spina bifida have difficulties with academic skills including math and reading comprehension. Although children with hydrocephalus (which is associated with a

9

number of neurological disorders, not just spina bifida) typically have good vocabulary and word attack skills, they have difficulty making inferences, understanding literal story content, and producing their own stories.

9

For no other group of exceptional learners is the continuum of educational services and placement options more relevant than for those with neuromotor or neurological disorders. Some students require a complex and coordinated array of specialized instruction, therapy, and related services. In addition, the transdisciplinary approach is beneficial for students with physical disabilities.

10

Environmental modifications are frequently necessary to allow a student to participate more fully and independently in school. Modifications include wheelchair-accessible classrooms or other assistive technology. IDEA defines assistive technology (AT) as devices and services needed to obtain and effectively use devices. A service is any resource that directly assists with the selection, acquisition, or use of an AT device. Adapted eating utensils, computerized speech devices, and telecommunication devices are all examples of AT.

One major consideration in the education of students with physical impairments is the physical environment of the classroom. For some students with mobility problems, this may be the prime area in which modifications must be made. Teachers must ensure that the physical layout is suitably arranged to accommodate students with physical impairments.

11

12

13

The effects of TBI on learning and behavior are determined by the severity of the injury and the part of the brain that sustained damage. Although people with brain injuries make significant improvements during the first two years post-injury and continue to improve at a more gradual pace for many years, most will have permanent physical, behavioral, and/or cognitive impairments.

14

15

Students with TBI experience a wide variety of learning and psychosocial characteristics. Moreover, physical and sensory changes also are common. The specific effect of the injury is dependent on the age of the student, the severity and location of the injury, and the time since the injury. Some individuals experience minimal changes, whereas others experience drastic changes. Learning and behavioral difficulties may persist long after the child has physically recovered.

Many children with TBI experience losses in language skills. Children who lose language functioning as a result of brain injury are said to have acquired aphasia. In other words, they have lost some language functions that they had acquired earlier. Language difficulties resulting from TBI can involve expressive language, receptive language, or both and can range from mild to severe. Two aspects of language have most frequently been reported to be affected by brain injuries: syntax and pragmatic skills. Nevertheless, many with TBI are able to recover much of their language functioning within 2 years after their injury.

In addition to impacting language and communication development, brain injuries can also have an impact on literacy skills. Children with traumatic brain injuries have been found to score significantly lower than age-, gender-, and race-matched uninjured children on achievement tests of reading, language, and mathematics.

16

Students who have experienced a TBI often re-enter school with deficits from their injuries compounded by their extended absence from school. These students are likely to require comprehensive programs of academic, psychological, and family support.

17

It is important that instructional strategies for students with TBI are carefully planned, systematically executed, and continuously monitored for effectiveness.

18

Ten elements that should be part of an educational program for children with TBI include:

1. Maximally controlled environment. The child may need a highly structured environment where distraction is reduced.

2. Low pupil–teacher ratio. It may be necessary to provide a classroom aide or other assistant to work with the child.

3. Intensive and repetitive instruction. The brain-injured child often needs more time to learn. Reducing nonacademic activities and lengthening the school year can provide more learning time.

4. Emphasis on process. The child may need to be helped in learning how to learn. Instruction should include help in sustaining attention and on memory.

5. Behavioral programming. Instructional strategies that use task analysis and careful measurement of progress have been found to be successful.

6. Integrated instructional therapies. Integrate allied therapies such as speech and physical therapy into the student’s primary instructional setting to facilitate generalization and transfer of skills.

7. Simulation experiences. Use simulations to enable the child to transfer skills to a new setting.

8. Cuing, fading, and shadowing. Students may require cues to respond, which should be faded as soon as possible. When shadowing, the teacher closely monitors the child

19

attempting a new task or moving to a new environment. 9. Readjustment counseling. This may help the child adjust to his or her new environment

and abilities. 10. Home-school liaison. It is essential to build and maintain a strong link between parents

and the school.

19

20

Get help from top-rated tutors in any subject.

Efficiently complete your homework and academic assignments by getting help from the experts at homeworkarchive.com