Deficit, Difference, or Both? Autism and Neurodiversity
Steven K. Kapp, Kristen Gillespie-Lynch, Lauren E. Sherman, and Ted Hutman University of California, Los Angeles
The neurodiversity movement challenges the medical model’s interest in causation and cure, celebrating autism as an inseparable aspect of identity. Using an online survey, we examined the perceived opposition between the medical model and the neurodiversity movement by assessing conceptions of autism and neurodiversity among people with different relations to autism. Participants (N � 657) included autistic people, relatives and friends of autistic people, and people with no specified relation to autism. Self-identification as autistic and neurodiversity awareness were associated with viewing autism as a positive identity that needs no cure, suggesting core differences between the medical model and the neurodiversity movement. Nevertheless, results suggested substantial overlap between these approaches to autism. Recognition of the negative aspects of autism and endorsement of parenting practices that celebrate and ameliorate but do not eliminate autism did not differ based on relation to autism or awareness of neurodiversity. These findings suggest a deficit-as-difference conception of autism wherein neurological conditions may represent equally valid pathways within human diversity. Potential areas of common ground in research and practice regarding autism are discussed.
Keywords: autism, neurodiversity, parenting, adaptation, identity
Many autistic people struggle with the difficulties associated with being autistic, viewing “difference” as a lonely experience of not belonging (e.g., Griffith, Totsika, Nash, & Hastings, in press; Humphrey & Lewis, 2008; Huws & Jones, 2008; Portway & Johnson, 2005; Ruiz Calzada, Pistrang, & Mandy, 2012), and some wish for a cure (Bagatell, 2010; Ortega, 2009; Punshon et al., 2009). However, autistic self-advocates within the neurodiversity, or autism rights, movement celebrate autism as inseparable from identity and challenge efforts to find a cause and a cure for it (Baker, 2011; Jaarsma & Welin, 2012; Jordan, 2010; Ortega, 2009).
The movement arose primarily on the Internet in response to the perceived marginalization of autistic people by organizations run by parents of autistic people (Chamak, 2008; Ortega, 2009). Pre- vious research has positioned neurodiversity and the medical model, which seeks to prevent and cure conditions like autism, in
binary opposition to one another, with parents of autistic people most commonly aligned with the medical model (Bagatell, 2010; Chamak, 2008; Clarke & van Amerom, 2008; Jordan, 2010; Orsini & Smith, 2010). This study aims to examine critically this oppo- sition by investigating how awareness of neurodiversity and rela- tionship to autism relate to three potential ways of responding to autism: elimination, amelioration, or celebration. Investigating these issues in terms of autism may shed light on how more generally to improve the quality of life of people on atypical developmental pathways.
Medical Model: Elimination and Amelioration
The medical model aspires toward normalization, symptom reduction, and elimination of conditions identified based on defi- cits said to cause functional impairment in major life activities (American Psychiatric Association, 2000; Baker, 2011). In the absence of biological markers, psychiatry mostly ascertains defi- cits on the basis of behavioral deviations from average (Anck- arsäter, 2010). This classification system tends to omit advanta- geous behaviors, the reasons for behaviors, and society’s role in determining appropriate behaviors (American Psychiatric Associ- ation, 2000; Armstrong, 2010; Baker, 2011). It thus does not distinguish between conditions resulting mainly from poor person– environment fit and diseases that cause deterioration and even death (Baker, 2011). By framing people with these conditions as sick or at least at reduced capacity, the medical model often confers the ability to make care decisions, especially for children and people considered severely disabled, upon professionals and family members (Baker, 2011; Silverman, 2012).
In apparent alignment with the medical model, many parents of autistic people pursue treatments for their child with the intention of cure, recovery, or at least a more normal appearance (Chamak, 2008). Many parents become knowledgeable about medical dis-
This article was published Online First April 30, 2012. Steven K. Kapp, Graduate School of Education and Information Studies,
University of California, Los Angeles; Kristen Gillespie-Lynch and Lauren E. Sherman, Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles; Ted Hutman, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sci- ences, University of California, Los Angeles.
The first two authors contributed equally and share primary authorship. This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant R01-
HD40432 to Scott P. Johnson and by the FPR-UCLA Center for Culture, Brain, and Development. We would like to thank participants of the study and people who helped with recruitment. David S. Smith played an instrumental role in the design of the survey. We are grateful to Patricia M. Greenfield and her lab, especially Yalda T. Uhls for her opportune intro- duction, for generous advice on the survey and the article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Steven K. Kapp, UCLA Graduate School of Education & Information Studies, 3132 Moore Hall, Box 951521, Los Angeles, CA 90095, or to Kristen Gillespie-Lynch, UCLA Department of Psychology, 2311 Franz Hall, Los Angeles, CA 90095. E-mail: [email protected] or [email protected]
Developmental Psychology © 2012 American Psychological Association 2013, Vol. 49, No. 1, 59 –71 0012-1649/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0028353
59
courses and practices, frequently delivering treatment as cothera- pists (Silverman, 2012). Parents and scientists focus their advo- cacy predominantly on children, partly because of the belief that treatments work most effectively when delivered early in life (Baker, 2011; Silverman, 2012). Some parents oriented toward the medical model have represented autism as hostile and distinct from the child they love, and themselves as warriors fighting an outside force holding their child hostage (Langan, 2011).
Indeed, many parents, professionals, and the lay public support the medical model by categorizing autism as a disease and even as an epidemic, based on the rise in number of diagnoses and belief in causal environmental factors (Hebert & Koulouglioti, 2010; Pellicano & Stears, 2011; Russell, Kelly, & Golding, 2010). Al- though expanded diagnostic criteria (American Psychiatric Asso- ciation, 2000) and rising awareness at least contribute to this increase in prevalence (Matson & Kozlowski, 2011), environmen- tal influences on autism’s causation suggest that the incidence of autism has also risen (e.g., Landrigan, 2010). Some parent advo- cates have used the epidemic claim to argue for unnatural causes like toxins; comparability with deadly diseases; and the urgent need to screen, treat, and try to eradicate sickness as a public health crisis (Baker, 2011). Following advocacy by relatives of autistic people, basic science research, which often relates to causation, has received the majority of autism research funding in the United States (Singh, Illes, Lazzeroni, & Hallmayer, 2009). Parental in- terest in understanding the cause of autism often reflects the belief that etiology will elucidate family planning and treatment (Pelli- cano & Stears, 2011).
Neurodiversity Movement: Celebration and Amelioration?
A political identity among autistic self-advocates, and disabled people more generally, positively relates to a proud identity and opposition to treatment toward a cure (Bagatell, 2010; Brownlow, 2010; Clarke & van Amerom, 2008; Hahn & Belt, 2004). Mirror- ing the concerns of other disabled people and activists (Madeo, Biesecker, Brasington, Erby, & Peters, 2011), many autistic self- advocates fear that cause-oriented research will lead to genetic prevention of autism (Baker, 2011; Orsini & Smith, 2010; Ortega, 2009; Pellicano & Stears, 2011). They also voice concern that prioritizing causation diverts resources from existing individuals (Pellicano & Stears, 2011; Robertson, 2010).
While neurodiversity proponents tend to adopt a form of the social model of disability, distinguishing between a biological, underlying condition or way of being (autism) and disability rooted substantially in inaccessible social and political infrastruc- tures (Baker, 2011), they essentialize autism as caused by biolog- ical factors and celebrate it as a part of natural human variation (Armstrong, 2010; Jaarsma & Welin, 2012; Ortega et al., 2009). Self-advocates often emphasize that autistic people’s insider ex- periences qualify them to lead attempts to remedy sociopolitical barriers and enable equal opportunity, such as by challenging negative conceptions of autism and improving accommodations and services (Baker, 2011; E. T. Savarese et al., 2010).
The neurodiversity movement seeks to provide a culture wherein autistic people feel pride in a minority group identity and provide mutual support in self-advocacy as a community (Baker, 2011; Jaarsma & Welin, 2012; Jordan, 2010; Ortega, 2009). View-
ing the strengths, differences, and weaknesses associated with autism as central to identity (Ne’eman, 2010; Robertson, 2010), self-advocates tend to prefer identity-first (e.g., “autistic person”) terms rather than the person-first (e.g., “person with autism”) language typically employed by the research community (Bagatell, 2010; Orsini & Smith, 2010; Ortega, 2009).
Neurodiversity advocates promote subjective well-being and adaptive rather than typical functioning, such as reliable, but not necessarily spoken, communication (Ne’eman, 2010; Robertson, 2010; E. T. Savarese et al., 2010; E. T. Savarese & Saverese, 2010). They oppose intervention that aims to eliminate unusual but harmless behaviors, like avoiding eye contact or repetitive body movements, across all contexts and without regard for the coping mechanisms they may serve (Chamak, 2008; Orsini & Smith, 2010; Ortega, 2009). Applied behavioral analysis (ABA) is one of the greatest sources of tension between many parents and self- advocates, who have criticized intensive behavioral interventions that they believe often focus too narrowly and forcefully on normalization for its own sake (Baker, 2011; Chamak, 2008; Ne’eman, 2010; Orsini & Smith, 2010; Ortega, 2009; Silverman, 2012).
In its pursuit of sociopolitical change and quality of life rather than cure, the neurodiversity movement has drawn controversy over to the extent to which it allows, if not encourages, ameliora- tion of autism. While emerging literature suggests that leaders of the neurodiversity movement acknowledge some deficits of autism and support some interventions to ameliorate them (Ne’eman, 2010; E. T. Savarese et al., 2010; E. T. Savarese & Saverese, 2010), others have interpreted the movement’s celebration of and opposition to elimination of autism as meaning that “high- functioning” self-advocates oppose diagnoses and interventions to ameliorate deficits (Clarke & van Amerom, 2008; Jaarsma & Welin, 2012; Tincani, Travers, & Boutot, 2009).
Deficit as Difference: Relations to Research Priorities
Differences between the research priorities of medical research- ers, parents of autistic individuals, and autistic self-advocates have led to a call for research that addresses the interests of parents and self-advocates (Pellicano & Stears, 2011). To our knowledge, no previous study has used the same measure to assess conceptions of autism among both the parents of autistic people and autistic people themselves. While much research has examined parental responses to autism, conceptions of autism held by autistic people and the lay public have received less attention (Huws & Jones, 2010; Pellicano & Stears, 2011). Learning about neurodiversity may serve as a turning point toward a more holistic conception of autism (Griffin & Pollak, 2009; King et al., 2003). Many parents come to feel strengthened by their child’s disability (Cappe, Wolff, Bobet, & Adrien, 2011; Meadan, Halle, & Ebata, 2010; Russell & Norwich, in press) and may become allies of the movement (Baga- tell, 2010; Langan, 2011; Ortega, 2009; R. J. Savarese et al., 2010). Increasing perception of positive aspects of autism may not de- crease recognition of negative aspects for both autistic self- advocates (Bagatell, 2010; Jones & Meldal, 2001; Punshon et al., 2009) and familial allies (R. J. Savarese et al., 2010).
The current study approaches three primary aims by assessing conceptions of autism and neurodiversity among people with dif- ferent relations to autism, including autistic people, parents of autistic people (some of whom are autistic themselves), nonparent
60 KAPP, GILLESPIE-LYNCH, SHERMAN, AND HUTMAN
relatives and friends of people on the spectrum, and people with no specified relationship to autism (a) to characterize awareness of and evaluations of the neurodiversity movement online (where the neurodiversity movement arose and often takes place; e.g., Jordan, 2010), (b) to confirm core distinctions between the medical model and the neurodiversity movement, and (c) to critically examine the perceived opposition between the medical model and the neurodi- versity movement.
Hypotheses of the Current Study
Awareness and Evaluations of the Neurodiversity Movement
We hypothesized that autistic people and their relatives would be more likely to be aware of neurodiversity than people with no relation to autism. Given that neurodiversity is enacted primarily online and generally by autistic people, we expected autistic peo- ple to be more likely to learn about it online and to define it less critically than others.
Expected Distinctions Between the Medical Model and Neurodiversity
Perceived causes and centrality to identity of autism. Be- cause autistic self-advocates oppose research on the cause of autism, while parents generally endorse such research, we expected autistic people and people aware of neurodiversity to be more likely to reject the validity of a question about the cause of autism and parents of autistic people to be less likely to do so. Because autistic self-advocates view autism as a natural part of themselves, we ex- pected autistic people and people aware of neurodiversity to be more likely to attribute autism to biology alone and to prefer an identity-first term for autism than their counterparts.
Deficit as Difference: Elucidating Distinctions and Overlaps Between the Neurodiversity Movement and the Medical Model
Perceived emotions about autism. Because neurodiversity awareness may serve as a turning point for autistic people, we expected autistic people and those aware of neurodiversity to endorse more positive emotions about autism than people with less contact with autism. Because negative emotions may be less sus- ceptible to change, we expected these factors to have no relation- ship with endorsement of negative emotions about autism.
Preferred parenting practices. Many of the tensions be- tween the neurodiversity movement and the medical model focus on aspects of parenting, such as acceptable goals and means of intervening. Accordingly, we wished to determine whether some parenting practices are endorsed regardless of awareness of neu- rodiversity, signaling overlap between deficit- and difference- oriented views of autism, and whether some parenting practices are differentially preferred based on neurodiversity awareness.
Given that autistic people, parents of autistic people, and neu- rodiversity proponents often celebrate autism yet recognize the importance of adaptive skills for autistic individuals, we expected these groups to be more supportive of parenting practices focused
on adapting to their child or understanding autism as part of their child’s identity but no less supportive of adaptive skills than their counterparts. Because autistic people and neurodiversity propo- nents are not often interested in eliminating autism, we expected them to be less supportive than other participants of parenting practices focused on finding a cause for and cure of autism and less supportive of services to help autistic people appear more typical.
Method
Participants
Ethical approval from a university-based institutional review board was obtained prior to recruitment of participants. An online survey was then posted on SurveyMonkey (http://www .surveymonkey.com). No compensation was provided for partici- pation. Before beginning the survey, participants completed an informed consent form online.
Recruitment was conducted through online advertisements and through e-mailed and mailed invitations to participate. Online advertisements were posted on autism-related (including for autis- tic people and parents of autistic people) and disability-related forums, blogs, and discussion lists, as well as disability-related groups on social networking sites (Facebook and Myspace). Ad- vertisements were also posted on Craigslist, an online classified advertisement community. All online recruitment sources based in physical locations were located in the United States or United Kingdom. Invitations to participate were e-mailed to members of autism advocacy and support groups located throughout the United States and United Kingdom. Invitations were also distributed to vocational rehabilitation centers, university disability offices, sec- ondary schools, and a disability youth advisory board, all located in the state of California. The researchers, one of whom is an autistic self-advocate, also recruited participants from their own social networks and e-mail lists and asked their contacts to redis- tribute the survey invitation.
An online survey was used, because the Internet overrepresents the activities and interests of both autistic self-advocates and parents who believe in and desire a cure for autism (Di Pietro, Whiteley, & Illes, in press; Jordan, 2010; Langan, 2011; Ortega, 2009; Reichow et al., in press; Stephenson, Carter, & Kemp, 2012). Efforts were made to recruit participants from numerous and diverse sources, including organizations that took explicit positions for or against curing autism (e.g., biomedical and inten- sive behavioral intervention-related organizations or autistic self- advocacy groups).
Participants who completed the survey (n � 657) represent a diverse group of people. They ranged in age from 8 to 84 years with a mean of 32.5 years. More participants were female, regard- less of diagnosis: 26.2% were male, 68.6% were female, and 3.5% were transgender or intersex. Because gender and autism were not independent of one another (see Table 1), transgender and intersex participants were dropped from analyses and gender was analyzed as a binary (male/female) variable. Education ranged from no education (0 years of schooling) to postdoctoral training (23 years of schooling) with a mean of 15.5 years. Relatively few partici- pants were ethnic minorities: 78.7% of the participants were Cau- casian, 4.6% were Hispanic, 2.7% were Asian, 1.8% were of African descent, .3% were Pacific Islander, and 6.1% were of
61AUTISM AND NEURODIVERSITY
mixed ethnicity. These percentages do not add up to 100% because some participants did not report their gender or ethnicity. Fourteen autistic participants did not know if they had received a diagnosis and thus were excluded from analysis. As can be seen in Table 1, participants who self-identified as autistic had more self-reported autistic traits on the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) than nonau- tistic participants. While no significant differences in autistic traits were apparent between autistic participants who had and had not received a formal diagnosis of autism, those who had received a formal diagnosis reported fewer years of education and were more frequently unemployed than nonautistic participants. Neither loca- tion of residence nor familial income was ascertained.
Survey Questions
Please see the Appendix for a complete list of survey questions. Demographics. Participants were asked to report gender, age,
highest level of education achieved, occupation, and ethnicity (see Table 1).
Relationship to autism. Participants were asked a series of questions to ascertain their relationship to autism. Based on these questions, participants were grouped into the following analytic cat- egories: “ASD diagnosed,” “ASD undiagnosed,” “parent of an autis- tic child,” “nonparent relative of an autistic individual,” “friend of an autistic individual,” or “person without contact with ASD.”
Autism Spectrum Quotient. The AQ is a 50-item self-report measure that assesses the number of autistic traits an individual exhibits. It has satisfactory internal consistency and test–retest reliability and can be used to evaluate where an individual falls along a continuum of sociocommunicative differences that extends into the general population (Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Skinner, Martin, & Clubley, 2001). Individuals on the autism spectrum often score above 26 on the AQ (Woodbury-Smith, Robinson, Wheelwright, & Baron-Cohen, 2005). For the purposes of the current study, the AQ was used only to verify that participants who identified themselves as autistic endorsed more autistic traits than those who did not self-identify as autistic.
Questions about neurodiversity. Participants were asked a series of questions to ascertain if and how they became aware of neurodiversity and what they thought neurodiversity was.
Questions about autism. Autism as identity. Participants were asked whether they
preferred the term “person with autism” or “autistic person.”
Emotions about autism. Autistic participants were asked to select emotions to characterize how they felt about autism. Multiple-choice answers were selected on the basis of pilot data. The frequency with which each participant endorsed positive (happy, proud, content, and excited) or negative (overwhelmed, sad, frustrated, angry, and ashamed) emotions about autism was calculated.
Attitudes toward parenting. Participants were asked how they felt autistic people should be parented.
Qualitative questions and coding. Regardless of previous awareness of neurodiversity, participants were asked to provide their own definition of neurodiversity: “What is the neurodiversity movement in your words?” Neurodiversity definitions were coded into mutually exclusive categories denoting their attitude. “Posi- tive/neutral valence” responses did not include any disparaging remarks or criticisms of the neurodiversity movement and may have included discussion of the strengths of the movement. “Mixed valence” responses provided both a neutral definition as well as a criticism, or discussed both strengths and weaknesses of the movement. “Negative valence” responses discussed only neg- ative aspects of the movement.
The first and third authors double-coded 132 of the responses for each item, representing 20% of the sample. The remainder of the responses was coded by the first author. Agreement on the classification of the valence of neurodiversity definitions was 100% (Cohen’s � � 1.0 on the valence of neurodiversity defini- tions).
Participants were also asked, “What do you think is the cause of autism?” Responses to this question were coded into mutually exclusive categories. “Biological” responses defined the cause of autism as genetic in nature or described specific aspects of the biological or neurological differences between autistic and typi- cally developing individuals. Responses categorized as “social environment” cited others’ behaviors or attitudes as the cause of autism, whereas responses categorized as “physical environment” cited nonhuman aspects of the environment, such as toxins or vaccines. Many individuals cited causes that fit multiple categories or simply described autism as having several causes; these re- sponses were categorized as “multiple causes.” Some participants did not cite a specific cause of autism. These responses, which were coded into the category “Validity Rejection,” described au- tism as part of the natural variation of human diversity or re-
Table 1 Demographics
Variable ASD-diagnosed ASD-undiagnosed Not ASD
N 223 78 342 AQ 35.32 (7.69) 36.77 (5.84) 16.30 (7.70) Dx/NDx � NA��
Age 30.80 (11.92) 35.19 (12.33) 33.28 (13.70) Education 14.86 (2.87) 15.78 (2.94) 15.96 (2.93) Dx � NA��
Unemployed (% yes) 14.3 12.8 2.9 Dx/NDx � NA�
Ethnicity (% White) 80.3 85.9 76.3 Gender (% transgender) 4.9 6.4 1.8 Gender (% male) 30.5 21.8 23.1 Medical conditions (% yes) 43.5 37.2 56.1 Dx/ND � NA�
Note. AQ � Autism Spectrum Quotient; ASD � autism spectrum disorder; Dx � ASD-diagnosed; NDx � ASD-undiagnosed; NA � Not ASD. Numbers are presented as mean (SD) except where % is noted. � � � .01. �� � � .001.
62 KAPP, GILLESPIE-LYNCH, SHERMAN, AND HUTMAN
sponded that they did not care about the cause of autism. Partici- pants who simply responded that they did not know the cause of autism, without providing a guess about the cause, were placed into the “other” category. Also in the “other” category were any responses that did not fit into the categories listed above, or responses in which the meaning was unclear. The inclusion of the “other” category allowed us to account for ambiguous responses. Agreement on the classification of cause statements was 91.1% (Cohen’s � � .88). Twenty percent of the responses to “What is your occupation?” were also coded by the first and third authors for employment or unemployment. Agreement on the classifica- tion of employment status was 98.48% (Cohen’s � � .83). Dis- agreements were resolved by discussion between the coders.
Results
The following demographic variables were included as covari- ates in all analyses: age, education, gender, and whether partici- pants endorsed nonautism diagnoses. In that context, we refer to independent variables evaluated in connection with our hypotheses as “primary” variables throughout this section. Because of the large number of analyses conducted, only p values under .01 were considered statistically significant, and all post hoc contrasts in- cluded Bonferroni corrections. In order to include demographic variables as covariates in all analyses, binomial logistic regression analysis was employed for categorical outcome variables, and the general linear model was employed for continuous outcome vari- ables.
Awareness and Evaluations of the Neurodiversity Movement
A binary logistic regression was conducted to determine if, over and above demographic characteristics, self-identification as au- tistic or the parent of an autistic child increased the likelihood of being aware of neurodiversity.
This analysis confirmed that autistic participants, regardless of diagnosis, were more likely to be aware of neurodiversity than
nonautistic participants. Being the parent of an autistic person was not associated with awareness of neurodiversity, but having an autistic friend was positively associated with awareness of neuro- diversity. Increased educational attainment was positively associ- ated with neurodiversity awareness.
Focusing on participants who reported that they were aware of neurodiversity, we conducted a binary logistic regression to deter- mine if, over and above demographic variables, self-identification as autistic increased the likelihood of learning about neurodiversity online (see Table 2).
As hypothesized, autistic participants, regardless of diagnosis, were more likely to have learned about neurodiversity online. Parents and those with other relationships to autism were not more likely to have learned about neurodiversity online.
Focusing on respondents who indicated that they were aware of neurodiversity, we used a binomial logistic regression to analyze predictors of attitudes toward neurodiversity, as indexed by the presence or absence of criticism of neurodiversity within their definitions of it. The overall model was not significant ( p � .096). Indeed, the majority of respondents provided uncritical definitions of neurodiversity. For participants in the current study, awareness of neurodiversity was generally associated with uncritical attitudes toward the movement. See Table 3 for the frequency with which each type of description of neurodiversity occurred.
As expected, autistic people and friends of autistic people, but contrary to expectations not relatives of autistic people, were more likely to be aware of neurodiversity than people with no relation to autism. Supporting previous qualitative research (e.g., Jordan, 2010) autistic people were more likely to learn about neurodiver- sity online than others. Contrary to our hypotheses, the majority of participants in the current study were uncritical of the neurodiver- sity movement, regardless of their relation to autism.
Expected Distinctions Between the Medical Model and the Neurodiversity Movement
Perceived causes and centrality to identity of autism. A binary logistic regression was run to determine if awareness of
Table 2 Predictors and Source of Neurodiversity Awareness
Variable
Predictors of neurodiversity awareness Learning about neurodiversity online
Odds ratio SE p Odds ratio SE p
ASD diagnosed 3.674 0.237�� �.001 6.061 0.343�� �.001 ASD undiagnosed 2.919 0.344� .002 10.827 0.570 �.001 Friend 3.271 0.200�� �.001 0.769 0.319 .410 Family ASD 1.412 0.249 .165 1.385 0.368 .376 Parent ASD 1.280 0.289 .393 1.650 0.395 .204 Other diagnosis 1.178 0.199 .411 2.132 0.300 .012 Age 1.010 0.010 .300 0.966 0.014 .014 Education 1.144 0.040� .001 0.992 0.059 .890 Gender 0.856 0.236 .512 0.429 0.355 .017 Constant 0.030 0.639�� �.001 2.842 0.974�� .283
Model �2 120.651�� �.001 65.615�� �.001 Cox & Snell R2 .201 .202 Nagelkerke R2 .269 .284
Note. ASD � autism spectrum disorder. � � � .01. �� � � .001.
63AUTISM AND NEURODIVERSITY
neurodiversity and self-identification as autistic were associated with greater likelihood of rejecting the validity of a question about the cause of autism, while self-identification as the parent of an autistic individual was associated with greater likelihood of pro- viding a cause (see Table 4).
Being the parent of an autistic child was negatively related to the likelihood of rejecting the validity of the question. Thus, parents viewed the cause of autism in a manner that was not consistent with the neurodiversity movement. Contrary to expectations, nei- ther awareness of neurodiversity nor self-identification as autistic was associated with likelihood of rejecting the validity of the question.
A binary logistic regression was run to determine if awareness of neurodiversity and self-identification as autistic were associated with greater likelihood of providing a purely biological cause for autism relative to a cause that attributed autism at least partially to
environmental input (social, physical, or multiple causes; see Ta- ble 4).
Self-identification as autistic, regardless of diagnosis, was asso- ciated with greater likelihood of selecting a biological cause while education was associated with greater likelihood of endorsing an environmental component.
We conducted a binary logistic regression analysis to determine whether awareness of neurodiversity and self-identification as autistic corresponded with increased likelihood of preferring an “identity- first” description of autism (“autistic person” rather than “person with autism”) beyond demographic characteristics (see Table 5).
Both self-identification as autistic, regardless of diagnosis, and awareness of neurodiversity were associated with a greater likeli- hood of preferring the term “autistic person” to the term “person with autism.” While autistic people and people who were aware of neurodiversity tended to prefer identity-first language, parents of
Table 3 Valence of Neurodiversity Definitions by Participants Aware of Neurodiversity
Valence “What is the neurodiversity movement in your words?” Examples of coding based on attitudes toward the neurodiveristy movement.
Positive or neutral 80.5% of those aware of neurodiversity provided this type of definition “We are all a spectrum and all different, it is not normal vs. disabled.” “A group that has taught me to accept my son EXACTLY for who he is.” “Accepting that people are different, that diversity in how our brains work enriches humankind.”
Mixed 3.4% of those aware of neurodiversity provided this type of definition “Sadly they seem angry that we want to help our sick children and act like we hate them if we do. Though I do think
there is a place for it and I am sure many ppl benefit from being part of a group the celebrates who they are.” “They want society to accept that we’re all different, but we’re all just human beings and we should all be accepted
for who we are. SOME in the neurodiversity movement however go to extremes, they want autistics to be treated SPECIAL, they make demands for changes in society that are a bit too rigorous and even silly in my opinion.”
“Inclusiveness, acceptance, a bit idealistic really.” Negative 1.8% of those aware of neurodiversity provided this type of definition
“A small group of people with a strong sense of entitlement and specialness.” “The idea that we autistic folks are not “abnormal,” just a different kind of normal. (This is bullshit.)” “A compendium of annoying adult children who need to adapt and stop finding pride in their inherent failure as
human beings.”
Table 4 Cause-of-Autism Items: (a) Validity Rejection Versus Providing a Cause and (b) Biological Versus Environmental Factors
Variable
(a) Validity rejection versus providing a cause (b) Biological versus environmental factors
Odds ratio SE p Odds ratio SE p
Neurodiversity awareness 2.051 0.438 .101 0.789 0.247 .337 ASD diagnosed 1.610 0.407 .242 2.802 0.263�� �.001 ASD undiagnosed 1.571 0.581 .437 3.378 0.374� .001 Friend 1.813 0.402 .139 1.024 0.241 .921 Family ASD 0.380 0.449 .031 0.950 0.273 .850 Parent ASD 0.150 0.667� .004 0.752 0.315 .365 Other diagnosis 2.371 0.375 .022 1.362 0.225 .169 Age 1.020 0.015 .199 0.990 0.011 .362 Education 0.966 0.071 .630 0.867 0.049� .004 Gender 0.872 0.408 .738 1.150 0.260 .592 Constant 0.031 1.166� .003 8.856 0.765�� .004
Model �2 27.874� .002 48.265�� �.001 Cox & Snell R2 .062 .116 Nagelkerke R2 .136 .155
Note. ASD � autism spectrum disorder. � � � .01. �� � � .001.
64 KAPP, GILLESPIE-LYNCH, SHERMAN, AND HUTMAN
autistic people and those with other types of relationships to autistic people did not have a preference for either term.
In apparent alignment with the medical model, parents were less likely to reject the validity of a question about the cause of autism than other participants. Unexpectedly, autistic participants and people aware of neurodiversity were not particularly likely to question its validity. In alignment with autistic self-advocates’ view of autism as a natural part of themselves (e.g., Ortega, 2009), autistic participants were more likely to attribute autism to purely biological causes, relative to causes with an environmental com- ponent, than other groups. Consistent with the neurodiversity movement’s view that autism is central to identity, autistic partic- ipants and people aware of neurodiversity were more likely to prefer the term “autistic person” to the term “person with autism” than their counterparts.
Deficit as Difference: Elucidating Distinctions and Overlaps Between the Neurodiversity Movement and the Medical Model
Perceived emotions about autism. In order to determine if positive emotions about autism varied as a function of neurodi-
versity awareness and relationship to autism, a univariate analysis was conducted with the number of positive emotions participants selected to describe how they would or did feel about being autistic as the dependent variable. Self-identification as autistic (a vari- able with three levels: autistic diagnosed, autistic undiagnosed, and not autistic), contact with autism (a variable with four levels: parent of autistic person, nonparent relative of autistic person, friend of autistic person, and no relationship with autistic person), neurodiversity awareness, and demographic variables were entered as independent variables.
There was a main effect of neurodiversity awareness, F(1, 476) � 7.366, p � .007, �2 � .015, and self-identification as autistic, F(2, 476) � 23.986, p � .001; �2 � .092, adjusted R2 � .247. People who were aware of neurodiversity (M � 1.084, SE � 0.083) endorsed more positive emotions about autism than partic- ipants who were not aware of neurodiversity (M � 0.593, SE � 0.098). Both diagnosed and undiagnosed autistic individuals en- dorsed more positive emotions about autism than nonautistic in- dividuals (see Table 6).
To examine negative emotions about autism, a univariate anal- ysis was conducted, with independent variables identical to those above and the number of negative emotions about autism endorsed as the dependent variable. No main effects or interactions were observed. Thus, awareness of neurodiversity and self- identification as autistic were related to positive but not negative emotions about autism. Being the parent of an autistic individual was unrelated to positive or negative emotions about autism.
Consistent with a nuanced view of the neurodiversity movement wherein recognition of the strengths of autism does not obscure understanding the difficulties associated with autism, self- identification as autistic and awareness of neurodiversity were associated with endorsing more positive, but not less negative, emotions about autism.
Preferred parenting practices. A multivariate analysis of covariance was run with the independent variables described for the univariate analyses above. The dependent variables can be viewed in the Appendix. Mean scores by autism identification can be viewed in Table 6.
Main effects of self-identifying as autistic, F(12, 928) � 2.758, p � .001, �2 � .035; of neurodiversity awareness, F(6, 463) �
Table 5 Predicting a Preference for an “Identity-First” Label
Variable Odds ratio SE p
Neurodiversity awareness 1.891 0.220� .004 ASD diagnosed 2.719 0.231�� �.001 ASD undiagnosed 2.895 0.332� .001 Friend 1.561 0.212 .035 Family ASD 0.926 0.244 .752 Parent ASD 0.916 0.303 .772 Other diagnosis 1.244 0.200 .277 Age 0.980 0.010 .053 Education 0.950 0.040 .198 Gender 1.025 0.234 .914 Constant 1.031 0.590 .959
Model �2 73.165�� �.001 Cox & Snell R2 .135 Nagelkerke R2 .182
Note. ASD � autism spectrum disorder. � � � .01. �� � � .001.
Table 6 Endorsement of Survey Questions by ASD Identification
Variable ASD diagnosed ASD undiagnosed Not ASD
Neurodiversity (% aware) 75.8 70.5 42.7 Neurodiversity (% online)a 85.2 89.1 49.3 Validity cause (% reject) 10.8 10.3 10.6 Cause (% purely biological) 46.2 51.3 28.4 Positive emotions 1.42 (1.25) 1.01 (0.99) 0.38 (0.81) Negative emotions 1.35 (1.49) 1.38 (1.29) 1.66 (1.45) Seek cureb 1.85 (1.18) 1.83 (1.11) 3.01 (1.31) Teach adaptive skillsb 4.62 (0.65) 4.54 (0.77) 4.69 (0.55) Teach appear typicalb 3.01 (1.30) 2.95 (1.22) 3.48 (1.07) Know autism part identityb 4.85 (0.46) 4.82 (0.50) 4.69 (0.68) Learn causeb 2.65 (1.30) 2.55 (1.20) 3.36 (1.21) Learn child’s languageb 4.70 (0.67) 4.60 (0.77) 4.53 (0.76)
Note. ASD � autism spectrum disorder. Numbers are presented as mean (SD) except where % noted. a Among people aware of neurodiversity. b Questions about parenting practices.
65AUTISM AND NEURODIVERSITY
3.203, p � .004, �2 � .040; and other medical conditions, F(6, 463) � 3.051, p � .006, �2 � .038, were observed.
Post hoc contrasts indicated that diagnosed autistic participants found it less important to try to understand the cause of one’s child’s autism than nonautistic participants (see Table 6; p � .003). Undiagnosed autistic participants did not differ from either diagnosed autistic or nonautistic participants in their interest in the cause of autism. Both diagnosed and undiagnosed autistic partic- ipants found it less important to seek a cure for one’s child’s autism than nonautistic participants (see Table 6; p � .001). People who were aware of neurodiversity (M � 2.042, SE � 0.098) were less interested in a cure for autism than those who were not (M � 2.864, SE � 0.117; p � .001). Despite the main effect of medical conditions for the overall multivariate analysis of variance, no significant post hoc effects of diagnosis were ob- served after Bonferroni correction.
As expected, no group differences in endorsement of parenting practices aimed at helping a child develop adaptive skills were observed. Consistent with the neurodiversity movement’s rejection of eliminating autism, autistic participants and people aware of neurodiversity found it less important for parents to try to seek a cure for autism than their counterparts. Contrary to expectations, awareness of neurodiversity was not associated with decreased interest in the cause of autism although self-identification as a diagnosed autistic was. Also contrary to expectations, autistic participants and those aware of neurodiversity were no less likely to support parenting practices aimed at helping autistic people appear typical and no more likely to endorse practices aimed at understanding autism as part of a child’s identity than their coun- terparts.
Discussion
Characterizing the Neurodiversity Movement Online
Autistic people were more likely to be aware of neurodiver- sity and to have learned about it online than nonautistic people. Many autistic people’s preferences for the Internet as a com- municative medium (Benford & Standen, 2009; Jordan, 2010) may have facilitated their learning about neurodiversity online. The generally uncritical definitions of the neurodiversity move- ment provided by participants in this study contrasts with previously reported criticisms of the neurodiversity movement (Bagatell, 2010; Baker, 2011; Chamak, 2008; Ortega, 2009). As it has become more political, the movement has achieved better representation in the media, public policy, and parent-led au- tism advocacy organizations (Baker, 2011; Nicolaidis et al., 2011; Pellicano & Stears, 2011; E. T. Savarese & Saverese, 2010; Silverman, 2012) and reached out more actively to allies (Baker, 2011; Nicolaidis et al., 2011; Orsini & Smith, 2010; Robertson, 2010). Additionally, the language and content of the survey may have led to its selective completion by people who were generally uncritical of the movement. Some participants may also have interpreted our question about the movement as an invitation to provide only a descriptive, rather than evalua- tive, definition.
Core Distinctions Between the Medical Model and the Neurodiversity Movement: Centrality to Identity and Opposition to Elimination
Results revealed clear distinctions between the medical model and the neurodiversity movement in terms of the per- ceived cause and importance of curing autism, positive emo- tions about autism, and the centrality of autism to identity. Formally diagnosed autistic participants expressed relative disin- terest in parental efforts to find a cause for autism, while parents were least likely to reject the validity of finding a cause. Autistic people may assign a lower priority to research on autism’s causa- tion because of concerns about genetic testing and worry that efforts to identify the cause may divert resources from services for existing autistic individuals (Baker, 2011; Orsini & Smith, 2010; Ortega, 2009; Pellicano & Stears, 2011) or because of a greater likelihood of attributing it to biology alone.
Contrary to both the social model of disability, wherein disabil- ity is socially constructed, and the medical model, wherein autism is generally viewed as arising from environmental and genetic causes (e.g., Pellicano & Stears, 2011), autistic individuals en- dorsed a relatively essentialist biological attribution of autism. While autistic people have referred to their brain as the obstacle preventing them from social acceptance (Humphrey & Lewis, 2008), becoming aware of their autism often offers them a sense of exoneration in explaining the neurological basis of their challenges (Punshon et al., 2009). Biological attributions may offer autistic people protection from the greater stigma associated with disabil- ities viewed as within one’s control (Hinshaw & Stier, 2008). The neurodiversity movement’s celebration of the brain may thus ap- peal to autistic people who likely already think of autism as a natural part of themselves.
Deficit as Difference: Celebration and Amelioration
The current study suggests that awareness of neurodiversity and self-identification as autistic correspond with a deficit-as- difference conception of autism. While both autistic identity and neurodiversity awareness were unrelated to negative emotions about autism and endorsement of the importance of helping a child build adaptive skills and— contrary to our expectations—appear more typical, both were associated with positive emotions about autism, a preference for identify-first language, and disinterest in a cure. These findings suggest self-identification as autistic and awareness of neurodiversity reduce neither acknowledgment of deficits associated with autism nor support for ameliorative inter- ventions, while they contribute to viewing autism as a positive identity that needs no cure. Such a deficit-as-difference conception of autism suggests the importance of harnessing autistic traits in developmentally beneficial ways, transcending a false dichotomy between celebrating differences and ameliorating deficits (R. J. Savarese et al., 2010).
The association between neurodiversity awareness and viewing autism as a positive identity may represent the convergence of social and medical model viewpoints. Positively reframing autism often helps parents of children with disabilities such as autism (e.g., Cappe et al., 2011; Hall, Neely-Barnes, Graff, Krcek, & Roberts, 2012; Meadan et al., 2010; Russell & Norwich, in press) and people with disabilities like autism (e.g., Clarke & van Am-
66 KAPP, GILLESPIE-LYNCH, SHERMAN, AND HUTMAN
erom, 2008; Jones & Meldal, 2001; Griffin & Pollak, 2009) cope. Reframing can consist of viewing autism as a difference rather than a deficit or of believing that autistic people will outgrow the problems associated with autism (Samios, Pakenham, & Sofronoff, 2008). The social model’s distinction between the condition and disability is not part of the medical model. Thus, an autistic person who has achieved a happy, productive, and independent life might be considered recovered in the medical model (Baker, 2011; Sil- verman, 2012) but living adaptively with and in part because of their autism in the social model (E. T. Savarese et al., 2010).
Although we expected autistic people, parents of autistic people, and people aware of neurodiversity to endorse celebration-related parenting practices more than their counterparts, most participants endorsed such practices. This may reflect recognition of the lack of a cure for autism and, hence, the practicality of recognizing it as part of identity. It may also reflect an understanding of the impor- tance of recognizing a child’s developmental level in order to help him or her expand upon it and that parental positive emotions about and acceptance of autism may not relate to child character- istics (Hutman, Siller, & Sigman, 2009; Milshtein, Yirmiya, Op- penheim, Koren-Karie, & Levi, 2010; Oppenheim, Koren-Karie, Dolev, & Yirmiya, 2009; Totsika, Hastings, Emerson, Lancaster, & Berridge, 2011; Wachtel & Carter, 2008).
The unexpected lack of differential endorsement of services to appear more typical, coupled with the predicted agreement on the importance of adaptive skills, suggest that autistic people and people aware of neurodiversity support at least some forms of behavioral interventions (e.g., R. J. Savarese et al., 2010). Like the false dichotomy between celebrating differences and ameliorating deficits, developmental and behavioral intervention approaches have shifted toward and can complement one another (Callahan, Shukla-Mehta, Magee, & Wie, 2010; Vismara & Rodgers, 2010). Callahan et al. (2010) found that parents and professionals reported equal satisfaction with the principles of ABA and another well- established model that claims to respect the “culture of autism” (TEACCH; Mesibov, Shea, & Shopler, 2004). Similarly, parent education programs using ABA that emphasize strengths rather than deficits appear to strengthen parent– child interaction (Steiner, 2011). Neurodiversity proponents have encouraged the use of interventions that leverage a person’s interests and strengths to address challenges positively (E. T. Savarese et al., 2010; R. J. Savarese et al., 2010). They have noted that restricted interests, a core symptom of autism (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), can, with support, enhance the social-communicative de- velopment of young children (R. J. Savarese et al., 2010) and mature into selective advantages (Armstrong, 2010; Brownlow, 2010).
Moreover, neurodiversity proponents have suggested the use- fulness of learning to appear more typical selectively as a coping strategy rather than an end in itself (Baker, 2011; Jones & Meldal, 2001), perhaps because the stigma of mental disabilities may reduce functioning more than the deficits (Hinshaw & Stier, 2008). Accordingly, even autistic people who support the ideals and long-term goals of the neurodiversity movement may view adapt- ing to a “neurotypical” world as a practical matter, given the slower pace of and less control over sociopolitical compared with personal change. Neurodiversity and disability rights advocates have likewise expressed acceptance of choice regarding identity, prevention, and cure based on comprehensive information that
includes disabled people’s views, abilities, and opportunities (Baker, 2011; Beauchamp-Pryor, 2011; Madeo et al., 2011).
Limitations
The online, self-selecting recruitment method and lack of de- tailed clinical information may bias the sample toward higher developmental and socioeconomic statuses relative to previous studies (e.g., Brugha et al., 2011) and thus limit generalizability of our results. More educated participants had higher awareness of neurodiversity, possibly suggesting less positive attitudes among people with less knowledge about it. Moreover, the autistic sample included a disproportionately large number of females (despite autism’s much higher prevalence among males; e.g., Kim et al., 2011) and people without formal diagnoses, groups at the margins, if not outside, of current and proposed diagnostic criteria for the autism spectrum (Frazier et al., 2012). A substantial proportion of autistic adults, especially females, with clear clinical histories may not present as autistic in behavioral diagnostic assessments adapted from childhood measures because they develop coping skills that superficially mask autism (Lai et al., 2011). Indeed, most people who meet diagnostic criteria for autism may be near the margins of a diagnosis, as recent studies on the prevalence of autism in total population community-based samples found that across the lifespan, most people who met criteria for ASD had not been previously diagnosed because of milder symptoms (Brugha et al., 2011; Kim et al., 2011; White, Ollendick, & Bray, 2011).
The sample may be more representative of the online autistic community and proponents of neurodiversity. Autistic females may be overrepresented online, as another recent online survey of autistic adults recruited an even higher female-to-male ratio (Gilmour, Schalomon, & Smith, 2012). They may disproportion- ately engage with the online community for social support and self-advocacy because of their greater difficulties in gaining rec- ognition as autistic (Jack, 2011). Many people claim an autistic identity through participation in online communities (Giles & Newbold, 2011; Jordan, 2010). Other reasons for the high number of informally diagnosed people could include difficulties directly diagnosing adults, accessing qualified professionals, and affording the evaluation, as well as expected problems with accessing ser- vices or accommodations if diagnosed. Future studies should ex- amine why some self-identified autistic people lack a diagnosis as well as differences between formally and informally diagnosed autistic people. To the extent that this study overrepresents fe- males, high-functioning autistic people, people who have self- diagnosed, and neurodiversity proponents, it provides evidence that they recognize deficits and support some ameliorative inter- ventions.
Future surveys of this kind will benefit from the development of a scale (the reliability and validity of which could be assessed) to evaluate conceptions of neurodiversity by including more ques- tions on each topic and evaluating the coherence of questions within each topic in order to permit analysis of the latent structure of the constructs. While the potential choices for the question about emotions about autism were selected on the basis of pilot data, the unequal number of positive, negative, and neutral emo- tions could have biased results. Additionally, asking directly whether participants were interested in understanding the cause of or finding a cure for autism may have been less confusing and
67AUTISM AND NEURODIVERSITY
more directly relevant than asking whether they thought parents should focus on such issues.
This study’s lack of nonacademic community members among its research team may have reduced sensitivity to participants’ diverse interests and needs. Despite clear indications in the instruc- tions that assistance could be offered to respondents who were unable to complete the survey independently, a shorter survey would have benefited people with limited language skills or less available time. While the survey’s topics and language may have discouraged people critical of the neurodiversity movement, crit- icisms of the AQ as lacking nuance from autistic participants suggest parts of the survey may have offended proponents of the movement. Indeed, when asked how the survey could have been improved, autistic participants expressed disappointment with our use of the AQ and concerns that we would use it to group them. They stated that it lacked nuance and upheld autism stereotypes— especially the controversial theory of autism as an extreme form of the male brain (Jack, 2011; Krahn & Fenton, 2012).
Deficit as Difference: Recommendations for Research Priorities
Autistic people, parents, and other parties may have relatively few absolute differences in their views about autism or neurodi- versity but, rather, disagree mainly on nuances too subtle for our survey to capture, such as research service priorities. Future stud- ies should focus more directly on the explicit research and service priorities of people with different relations to autism in order to tailor research and services to the needs of stakeholders. They should recruit both online and offline and incorporate community- based participatory research that includes autistic people, parents, practitioners, and researchers in every step of the research process (Ne’eman, 2010; Nicolaidis et al., 2011; Orsini & Smith, 2010; Pellicano & Stears, 2011; Robertson, 2010). Such research could develop methods for studying a broader range of autistic and nonautistic people while combining scientific rigor with commu- nity needs. The results of this study suggest potential for collab- orative research to find common ground on best practices in providing interventions and services to help autistic people and their families across the lifespan. If future, more generalizable research replicates this study’s finding that officially diagnosed autistic people have less interest in the cause of autism, a higher proportion of research funding may shift toward interventions and services as the interests of autistic people and the objectives of the neurodiversity movement become better represented in public pol- icy. Indeed, this shift may have already begun. Parent-led advo- cacy organizations’ proportion of funding of basic science and causation research has dropped compared with funding of clinical and translational research (Singh et al., 2009).
Community-based participatory research should examine the movement’s breadth beyond autism (Beauchamp-Pryor, 2011). Conceptually, many neurological conditions have variable traits, fluid boundaries among one another, a continuous nature within the general population, and strengths beyond or as part of signif- icant challenges (Anckarsäter, 2010; Armstrong, 2010). As autistic self-advocates relate the brain to both the mind (cognition and emotions) and the body (sensation and movement), neurodiversity appears applicable beyond mental conditions (Robertson, 2010; E. T. Savarese et al., 2010). Nevertheless, neurodiversity propo-
nents disagree on criteria for eligibility in the broader movement; some autistic advocates suggest aversion to conditions that revolve around distress (Ne’eman, 2010; E. T. Savarese et al., 2010), while allies and scholars have included them (Armstrong, 2010; Baker, 2011; E. T. Savarese & Saverese, 2010). Similarly, disability rights advocates often think the social model does not apply to pain and chronic illness (Beauchamp-Pryor, 2011). Politically, the move- ment may have greater appeal among “invisible” conditions with unknown causes, given the belief that constructing a biological identity reduces judgment and improves access to services (Baker, 2011; Orsini & Smith, 2010), and among conditions with early age of onset, which is positively associated with disability pride (Beauchamp-Pryor, 2011; Hahn & Belt, 2004).
Conclusion
This study provides support for the notion of disability as an interaction between social factors and personal deficits, the chal- lenges of which do not necessarily make life less valid or worth- while but an equally valid part of human diversity, especially in the subjective experience of disabled people. Considering that autism is diagnosed primarily on the basis of social deficits (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), autistic people’s apparent ac- knowledgment of their deficits and acceptance of means to ame- liorate them challenge a purely social model of disability in which oppression alone creates disability, a notion disability rights ad- vocates increasingly criticize as not recognizing that deficits them- selves lower quality of life (Beauchamp-Pryor, 2011; Palmer & Harley, in press). Neurodiversity advocates, while often empha- sizing social barriers, have acknowledged this interrelationship between internal and social challenges (Baker, 2011; Ne’eman, 2010).
Indeed, an international biopsychosocial model of causation of and support for disability now prevails (Leckman & March, 2011; Palmer & Harley, in press). This emerging, nuanced understanding of disability may require disentanglement of symptoms and adap- tive functioning (Anckarsäter, 2010) and care supporting signifi- cantly challenged people, including considering the perspectives, abilities, and opportunities of people with disabilities (Baker, 2011; Beauchamp-Pryor, 2011; Madeo et al., 2011; E. T. Savarese & Savarese, 2010; Silverman, 2012).
Nevertheless, the spectrum nature of disability supports the legitimacy of multiple agendas (Baker, 2011). Scientists, working with the community, can help stakeholders with competing agen- das make informed choices between rights, responsibilities, and needs at personal, social, and political levels by affirming that diverse societies respect multiple perspectives (Baker, 2011; Beauchamp-Pryor, 2011; Madeo et al., 2011; Silverman, 2012), as empathy, communication, and relationship work both ways (E. T. Savarese et al., 2010; Silverman, 2012).
References
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical man- ual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
Anckarsäter, H. (2010). Beyond categorical diagnostics in psychiatry: Scientific and medicolegal implications. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 33, 59 – 65. doi:10.1016/j.ijlp.2009.12.001
Armstrong, T. (2010). Neurodiversity: Discovering the extraordinary gifts
68 KAPP, GILLESPIE-LYNCH, SHERMAN, AND HUTMAN
of autism, ADHD, dyslexia, and other brain differences. Cambridge, MA: Da Capo.
Bagatell, N. (2010). From cure to community: Transforming notions of autism. Ethos, 38, 34 –58. doi:10.1111/j.1548-1352.2009.01080.x
Baker, D. L. (2011). The politics of neurodiversity: Why public policy matters. Boulder, CO: Lynne Rienner.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17. doi:10.1023/A:1005653411471
Beauchamp-Pryor, K. (2011). Impairment, cure and identity: “Where do I fit in?” Disability & Society, 26, 5–17. doi:10.1080/ 09687599.2011.529662
Benford, P., & Standen, P. J. (2009). The Internet: A comfortable com- munication medium for people with Asperger syndrome (AS) and high functioning autism (HFA)? Journal of Assistive Technologies, 3, 44 –53. doi:10.1108/17549450200900015
Brownlow, C. (2010). Presenting the self: Negotiating a label of autism. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 35, 14 –21. doi: 10.3109/13668250903496336
Brugha, T. S., McManus, S., Bankart, J., Scott, F., Purdon, S., Smith, J., . . . Meltzer, H. (2011). Epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders in adults in the community in England. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 459 – 466. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.38
Callahan, K., Shikla-Mehta, S., Magree, S., & Wie, M. (2010). ABA versus TEACCH: The case for defining and validating comprehensive treat- ment models in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- ders, 40, 74 – 88. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0834-0
Cappe, E., Wolff, M., Bobet, R., & Adrien, J. L. (2011). Quality of life: A key variable to consider in the evaluation of adjustment of parents of children with autism spectrum disorders and in the development of relevant support and assistance programs. Quality of Life Research, 20, 1279 –1294. doi:10.1007/s11136-011-9861-3
Chamak, B. (2008). Autism and social movements: French parents’ asso- ciations and international autistic individuals’ organizations. Sociology of Health & Illness, 30, 76 –96. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9566.2007.01053.x
Clarke, J., & van Amerom, G. (2008). Asperger’s syndrome: Differences between parents’ understanding and those diagnosed. Social Work in Health Care, 46, 85–106. doi:10.1300/J010v46n03_05
Di Pietro, N. C., Whiteley, L., & Illes, J. (in press). Treatments and services for neurodevelopmental disorders on advocacy websites: Information or evaluation? Neuroethics.
Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Speer, L., Embacher, R., Law, P., Constantino, J., . . . Eng, C. (2012). Validation of proposed DSM–5 criteria for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 28 – 40. doi:10.1016/j.jaac .2011.09.021
Giles, D. C., & Newbold, J. (2011). Self- and other-diagnosis in user-led online mental health communities. Qualitative Health Research, 21, 419 – 428. doi:10.1177/1049732310381388
Gilmour, L., Schalomon, P. M., & Smith, V. (2012). Sexuality in a community based sample of adults with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 313–318. doi:10.1016/ j.rasd.2011.06.003
Griffin, E., & Pollak, D. (2009). Student experiences of neurodiversity in higher education: Insights from the BRAINHE Project. Dyslexia, 15, 23– 41. doi:10.1002/dys.383
Griffith, G. M., Totsika, V., Nash, S., & Hastings, R. P. (in press). “I just don’t fit anywhere”: Support experiences and future support needs of individuals with Asperger syndrome in middle adulthood. Autism.
Hahn, H. D., & Belt, T. L. (2004). Disability identity and attitudes toward cure in a sample of disabled activists. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 45, 453– 464. doi:10.1177/002214650404500407
Hall, H. R., Neely-Barnes, S. L., Graff, J. C., Krcek, T. E., & Roberts, R. J. (2012). Parental stress in families of children with a genetic disorder/ disability and the resiliency model of family stress, adjustment, and adaptation. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 35, 24 – 44. doi:10.3109/01460862.2012.646479
Hebert, E. B., & Koulouglioti, C. (2010). Parental beliefs about cause and course of their child’s autism and outcomes of their beliefs: A review of the literature. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 33, 149 –163. doi:10.3109/01460862.2010.498331
Hinshaw, S. P., & Stier, A. (2008). Stigma as related to mental disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 367–393. doi:10.1146/ annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141245
Humphrey, N., & Lewis, S. (2008). “Make me normal”: The views and experiences of pupils on the autistic spectrum in mainstream secondary schools. Autism, 12, 23– 46. doi:10.1177/1362361307085267
Hutman, T., Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2009). Mothers’ narratives regard- ing their child with autism predict maternal synchronous behavior during play. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 1255–1263. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02109.x
Huws, J. C., & Jones, R. S. P. (2008). Diagnosis, disclosure, and having autism: An interpretative phenomenological analysis of the perceptions of young people with autism. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 33, 99 –107. doi:10.1080/13668250802010394
Huws, J. C., & Jones, R. S. P. (2010). “They just seem to live their lives in their own little world”: Lay perceptions of autism. Disability & Society, 25, 331–344. doi:10.1080/09687591003701231
Jaarsma, P., & Welin, S. (2012). Autism as a natural human variation: Reflections on the claims of the neurodiversity movement. Health Care Analysis, 20, 20 –30. doi:10.1007/s10728-011-0169-9
Jack, J. (2011). “The extreme male brain?” Incrementum and the rhetorical gendering of autism. Disability Studies Quarterly, 31. Retrieved from http://dsq-sds.org/
Jones, R., & Meldal, T. O. (2001). Social relationships and Asperger’s syndrome: A qualitative analysis of first-hand accounts. Journal of Intellectual Disabilities, 5, 35– 41. doi:10.1177/146900470100500104
Jordan, C. J. (2010). Evolution of autism support and understanding via the World Wide Web. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 48, 220 –227. doi:10.1352/1934-9556-48.3.220
Kim, Y. S., Leventhal, B. T., Koh, Y.-J., Fombonne, E., Laska, E., Lim, E. C., . . . Grinker, R. R. (2011). Prevalence of autism spectrum disor- ders in a total population sample. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 904 –912. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10101532
King, G., Cathers, T., Brown, E., Specht, J. A., Willoughby, C., Polgar, J. M., . . . Havens, L. (2003). Turning points and protective processes in the lives of people with chronic disabilities. Qualitative Health Re- search, 13, 184 –206. doi:10.1177/1049732302239598
Krahn, T. M., & Fenton, A. (2012). The extreme male brain theory of autism and the potential adverse effects for boys and girls with autism. Bioethical Inquiry, 9, 93–103. doi:10.1007/s11673-011-9350-y
Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., Pasco, G., Ruigrok, A. V., Wheelwright, S. J., Sadek, S. A., . . . Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). A behavioral compar- ison of male and female adults with high functioning autism spectrum conditions. PLoS ONE, 6, e20835. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0020835
Landrigan, P. J. (2010). What causes autism? Exploring the environmental contribution. Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 22, 219 –225. doi:10.1097/ MOP.0b013e328336eb9a
Langan, M. (2011). Parental voices and controversies in autism. Disability & Society, 26, 193–205. doi:10.1080/09687599.2011.544059
Leckman, J. F., & March, J. S. (2011). Editorial: Developmental neuro- science comes of age. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 333–338. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02378.x
Madeo, A. C., Biesecker, B. B., Brasington, C., Erby, L. H., & Peters, K. F. (2011). The relationship between the genetic counseling profession and
69AUTISM AND NEURODIVERSITY
the disability community: A commentary. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 155A, 1777–1785. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.34054
Matson, J. L., & Kozlowski, A. M. (2011). The increasing prevalence of autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 418 – 425. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2010.06.004
Meadan, H., Halle, J. W., & Ebata, A. T. (2010). Families with children who have autism spectrum disorders: Stress and support. Exceptional Children, 77, 7–36.
Mesibov, G. B., Shea, V., & Shopler, E. (2004). The TEACCH approach to autism spectrum disorders. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Milshtein, S., Yirmiya, N., Oppenheim, D., Koren-Karie, N., & Levi, S. (2010). Resolution of the diagnosis among parents of children with autism spectrum disorder: Associations with child and parent character- istics. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 89 –99. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0837-x
Ne’eman, A. (2010). The Future (and the past) of autism advocacy, or why the ASA’s magazine, The Advocate, wouldn’t publish this piece. Dis- ability Studies Quarterly, 30. Retrieved from http://www.dsq-sds.org/
Nicolaidis, C., Raymaker, D., McDonald, K., Dern, S., Ashkenazy, E., Boisclair, C., . . . Baggs, A. (2011). Collaboration strategies in nontra- ditional community-based participatory research partnerships: Lessons from an academic-community partnership with autistic self-advocates. Progress in Community Health Partnerships: Research, Education, and Action, 5, 143–150. doi:10.1353/cpr.2011.0022
Oppenheim, D., Koren-Karie, N., Dolev, S., & Yirmiya, N. (2009). Ma- ternal insightfulness and resolution of the diagnosis are associated with secure attachment in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. Child Development, 80, 519 –527. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01276.x
Orsini, M., & Smith, M. (2010). Social movements, knowledge and public policy: The case of autism activism in Canada and the US. Critical Policy Studies, 4, 38 –57. doi:10.1080/19460171003714989
Ortega, F. (2009). The cerebral subject and the challenge of neurodiversity. BioSocieties, 4, 425– 445. doi:10.1017/S1745855209990287
Palmer, M., & Harley, D. (in press). Models and measurement in disability: An international review. Health Policy and Planning.
Pellicano, E., & Stears, M. (2011). Bridging autism, science, and society: Moving toward an ethically informed approach to autism research, Autism Research, 4, 271–282. doi:10.1002/aur.201
Portway, S. M., & Johnson, B. (2005). Do you know I have Asperger’s syndrome? Risks of a non-obvious disability. Health, Risk & Society, 7, 73– 83. doi:10.1080/09500830500042086
Punshon, C., Skirrow, P., & Murphy, G. (2009). The “not guilty verdict”: Psychological reactions to a diagnosis of autism in adulthood. Autism, 13, 265–283. doi:10.1177/1362361309103795
Reichow, B., Halpern, J. I., Steinhoff, T. B., Letsinger, N., Naples, A., & Volkmar, F. R. (in press). Characteristics and quality of autism websites. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.
Robertson, S. M. (2010). Neurodiversity, quality of life, and autistic adults: Shifting research and professional focuses onto real-life challenges. Disability Studies Quarterly, 30. Retrieved from http://www.dsq- sds.org/
Ruiz Calzada, L., Pistrang, N., & Mandy, W. P. (2012). High-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder: Utility and meaning for families. Jour- nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 230 –243. doi:10.1007/ s10803-011-1238-5
Russell, G., Kelly, S., & Golding, J. (2010). A qualitative analysis of lay beliefs about the aetiology and prevalence of autism spectrum disorders.
Child: Care, Health, and Development, 36, 431– 436. doi:10.1111/ j.1365-2214.2009.00994.x
Russell, G., & Norwich, B. (in press). Dilemmas, diagnosis, and de- stigmatization: Parental perspectives on the diagnosis of autism spec- trum disorders. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry.
Samios, C., Pakenham, K. I., & Sofronoff, K. (2008). The nature of sense making in parenting a child with Asperger syndrome. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 516 –532. doi:10.1016/j.rasd .2007.10.003
Savarese, E. T., Baggs, A., Burke, J., Kochmeister, S., Rubin, S., Schwarz, P., . . . Tresher, T. (2010). Self-advocate roundtable with members of AutCom. Disability Studies Quarterly, 30. Retrieved from http://dsq- sds.org/
Savarese, E. T., & Savarese, R. J. (2010). “The superior half of speaking”: An introduction. Disability Studies Quarterly, 30. Retrieved from http:// dsq-sds.org/
Savarese, R. J., Block, P., Chew, K., Cumberland, C., Duffy, J., Etlinger, S., . . . Wilson, J. C. (2010). Parent & sibling roundtable: Neurodiversity and caregiving. Disability Studies Quarterly, 30. Retrieved from http:// www.dsq-sds.org/
Silverman, C. (2012). Understanding autism: Parents, doctors, and the history of the disorder. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Singh, J. Illes, J., Lazzeroni, L., & Hallmayer, J. (2009). Trends in US autism research funding. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- ders, 39, 788 –795. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0685-0
Steiner, A. M. (2011). A strength-based approach to parent education for children with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 13, 178 –190. doi:10.1177/1098300710384134
Stephenson, J., Carter, M., & Kemp, C. (2012). Quality of the information on educational and therapy interventions provided on the web sites of national autism associations. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 11–18. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2011.08.002
Tincani, M., Travers, J., & Boutot, A. (2009). Race, culture, and autism spectrum disorder: Understanding the role of diversity in successful educational interventions. Research and Practice for Persons with Se- vere Disabilities, 34, 81–90.
Totsika, V., Hastings, R. P., Emerson, E., Lancaster, G. A., & Berridge, D. M. (2011). A population-based investigation of behavioural and emotional problems and maternal mental health: Associations with au- tism spectrum disorder and intellectual disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 91–99. doi:10.1111/j.1469- 7610.2010.02295.x
Vismara, L. A., & Rogers, S. J. (2010). Behavioral treatments in autism spectrum disorder: What do we know? Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 447– 468. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131151
Wachtel, K., & Carter, A. S. (2008). Reaction to diagnosis and parenting styles among mothers of young children with ASDs. Autism, 12, 575– 594. doi:10.1177/1362361308094505
White, S. W., Ollendick, T. H., & Bray, B. C. (2011). College students on the autism spectrum: Prevalence and associated problems. Autism, 15, 683–701. doi:10.1177/1362361310393363
Woodbury-Smith, M. R., Robinson, J., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). Screening adults for Asperger syndrome using the AQ: A preliminary study of its diagnostic validity in clinical practice. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 331–335. doi:10.1007/ s10803-005-3300-7
70 KAPP, GILLESPIE-LYNCH, SHERMAN, AND HUTMAN
Appendix
Survey Questions and Answers
Demographic Questions
1. Do you consider yourself to be autistic or on the autism spectrum (autism, Aspergers, or PDD-NOS)?
Answer choices: Yes, No
2. Asked of participants that self-identified as autis- tic: “Were you diagnosed by a professional such as a psychologist, doctor or psychiatrist?”
Answer choices: Yes, No
3. Do you have any autistic relatives? If so, please list how they are related to you (i.e., a grandmother, a brother, etc.).
Free response
4. Do you have any autistic friends?
Answer choices: Yes, No
5. What is your gender?
Answer choices: Male, Female, Intersex, Transgender
6. How old are you?
Free response
7. What is the highest level of education you achieved?
Free response
8. What is your ethnicity?
Free-response ethnicity reports were classified into the following race and ethnicity categories: Caucasian, Black, Asian, Hispanic, Native American or Alaska Native, Pacific Islander, or Mixed Race.
9. Do you have any physical, neurological, or psy- chological diagnoses? If so, what are they?
Free-response answers were classified as “medical condi- tions” if any health condition besides an ASD was entered.
10. What is your occupation?
Free response
Conceptions of Neurodiversity
1. Are you aware of the neurodiversity movement? If yes, where did you learn about it?
Answer choices: “No, I am not aware of it”; “Yes, I heard of it online”; “Yes, I read about it in a book or magazine”; “Yes, I heard of it in person”; “Yes, I heard about it at a conference”; “Yes, I heard about it at a support group”; “Yes, but none of the above.”
2. What is the neurodiversity movement in your words?
Free response
Conceptions of Autism
1. When talking about autism, which term do you prefer?
Answer choices: Autistic person, person with autism
2. How do you (think you would) feel about being autistic? Select as many choices as you want.
Answer choices: “Happy,” “overwhelmed, “sad,” “proud,” “frustrated,” “angry,” “content,” “indifferent,” “bored,” “confused,” “ashamed,” “excited,” “other,” and “don’t know.”
3. Do you agree or disagree that parents of autistic people should do the following:
“Seek a cure for their child?”
“Teach their child how to develop adaptive skills?”
“Teach their child how to appear more like a typically developing person?”
“Understand that autism is part of their child’s identity?”
“Try to learn what caused their child to be autistic?”
“Learn to speak their child’s language?”
Answer choices (1–5): “I strongly disagree,” “I somewhat disagree,” “Not applicable,” “I agree,” “I strongly agree.”
4. What do you think is the cause of autism?
Free response
Received May 16, 2011 Revision received March 9, 2012
Accepted March 16, 2012 �
71AUTISM AND NEURODIVERSITY
Children’s Racial Categorization in Context
Kristin Pauker, 1 Amanda Williams,
2 and Jennifer R. Steele
3
1 University of Hawaii,
2 Sheffield Hallam University, and
3 York University
ABSTRACT—The ability to discriminate visually based on
race emerges early in infancy: 3-month-olds can percep-
tually differentiate and 6-month-olds can perceptually
categorize faces by race. Between ages 6 and 8 years,
children can sort others into racial groups. But to what
extent are these abilities influenced by context? In this
article, we review studies on children’s racial categoriza-
tion and discuss how our conclusions are affected by how
we ask the questions (i.e., our methods and stimuli),
where we ask them (i.e., the diversity of the child’s sur-
rounding environment), and whom we ask (i.e., the diver-
sity of the children we study). Taken together, we suggest
that despite a developmental readiness to categorize
others by race, the use of race as a psychologically salient
basis for categorization is far from inevitable and is
shaped largely by the experimental setting and the greater
cultural context.
KEYWORDS—racial categorization; racial stereotyping and
prejudice; social development
Racial prejudice is one of the most pressing social issues of our
time. Social and developmental psychologists have sought to
understand more deeply when racial biases emerge in child-
hood. Despite the foundational role of racial categorization in
stereotyping and prejudice, research with children has focused
almost exclusively on the downstream consequences of racial
categorization rather than the process of racial categorization
itself. In this article, we review what is known about racial cate-
gorization from infancy into late childhood, with a focus on
recent research. In addition, we argue that researchers need to
devote greater attention to the experimental setting and the lar-
ger cultural context to advance our theoretical and practical
understanding of the development of racial categorization.
WHEN CAN CHILDREN CATEGORIZE BY RACE?
The answer to this question depends largely on how categoriza-
tion is defined. For example, does noticing differences between
racial groups, sorting targets with similar skin color together,
identifying physical features as typical of group members, or
labeling members of different racial groups provide sufficient
evidence of racial categorization? In this article, we define racial
categorization as the tendency for race to be perceived as a psy-
chologically salient and meaningful basis for grouping others.
This definition builds on the developmental intergroup theory
(DIT; 1), in which four main factors contribute to the psychologi-
cal salience of social categories: (a) perceptual salience (i.e.,
whether categories are marked by discriminable visual features),
(b) proportional group size (i.e., proportionally smaller groups, or
minorities, tend to be more distinct), (c) explicit labeling by
adults (e.g., “the Black child”), which suggests the dimension
merits attention and provides a category label, and (d) implicit
use in the environment (e.g., through racial segregation of neigh-
borhoods), which may lead children to independently construct
explanations regarding the importance of shared attributes (1).
Measuring racial categorization involves administering tasks that
map onto these factors, and exploring how and when children
consistently and spontaneously use the category to organize
information and direct behavior. This definition of racial catego-
rization highlights not only how many inputs (both perceptual
and conceptual) integrate to inform children’s categorizations,
but also how context directs whether race is salient psychologi-
cally and thus used habitually in a psychologically meaningful
way. Although outside the scope of this article, one important
conceptual input into children’s categorizations is their intuitive
theories, including beliefs that social categories are natural
Kristin Pauker, University of Hawaii; Amanda Williams, Sheffield Hallam University; Jennifer R. Steele, York University.
This work was supported by a grant from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R00-HD065741) to Kristin Pauker.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kristin Pauker, Department of Psychology, University of Hawaii, 2530 Dole St., Sakamaki C400, Honolulu, HI 96822; e-mail: [email protected].
© 2015 The Authors
Child Development Perspectives © 2015 The Society for Research in Child Development
DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12155
Volume 10, Number 1, 2016, Pages 33–38
CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES
kinds (2). Yet even these intuitive theories may be shaped by
cultural context (2–4). Although some factors contributing to the psychological salience of race can emerge quite early in infancy
(e.g., perceptual discrimination) and other components depend
more on linguistic skills that develop later in childhood (e.g.,
labeling by race), all are influenced by both the immediate
(experimental) and broader (cultural) context.
Infants
Although infants are not attuned to racial differences at birth
(5), their ability to differentiate perceptually based on race
develops early in homogeneous cultural contexts. By 3 months,
White, Black, and Asian infants from countries where their race
is the majority (i.e., White infants in the United Kingdom, Black
infants in Ethiopia, and Asian infants in China) look longer at
same-race faces than at other-race faces (5–7). However, despite this visual preference for same-race faces, young infants do not
show impaired recognition of other-race faces that is typically
seen in adults (8). Instead, at 3 months, White and Asian
infants from countries where their race is the majority can rec-
ognize different faces of their race as well as different faces of
other races (9, 10). These infants demonstrate a decreasing abil-
ity to differentiate other-race faces across many out-groups
between 3 and 9 months, and by 9 months, they recognize
same-race faces but have difficulty recognizing other-race faces
(9, 10), similar to the impaired ability to recognize other-race
faces seen in adults (8).
Thus, while 3-month-olds raised in homogenous cultural con-
texts show sensitivity to distinctions between racial groups, they
can still individuate faces within racial groups. However, the
ability to individuate within racial groups apparently changes
with development and environmental input—and children become tuned to the faces they encounter most frequently as
they age. Consistent with the strong connection in adults
between categorical processing of race and impaired recognition
of other-race faces (8), this perceptual tuning also apparently
coincides with infants’ ability to categorize faces by race (11).
Infants can perceptually categorize some faces by race at
6 months (12): Specifically, in one study, when White 6-month-
olds with limited exposure to other-race faces were familiarized
with many Black or Asian faces (i.e., faces belonging to a single
racial category), they distinguished between a new face from
the familiarized racial category compared to a new face from a
novel racial category (i.e., Asian or Black, respectively; 12).
This design tests whether infants categorized a new face from
the familiarized category as part of the same category and a face
from the novel racial category as part of a different category.
However, at 9 months, White infants no longer distinguished
between many other-race categories, instead forming a broader
distinction between same-race (White = in-group) and other- race faces grouped together (Asian and Black = out-group; 12). In all the studies with infants we have reviewed, stimuli
consisted of color photographs of faces that used both facial
features and skin tone as visual markers of race. Thus, we can-
not determine whether infants use one or both of these visual
cues to process same- and other-race faces. However, in some
studies (13), the ability to differentiate same- and other-race
faces was not necessarily based solely on low-level perceptual
cues such as skin color. When presented with computer-gener-
ated faces that depicted prototypical physiognomy and skin
tone (i.e., Eurocentric facial features with White skin tone and
Afrocentric features with Black skin tone) or faces that isolated
these aspects (e.g., Eurocentric features with Black skin tone
and Afrocentric features with White skin tone), the neural
responses of White majority 9-month-olds in the United States
did not differ when viewing prototypical White faces in com-
parison to faces that isolated Black features (i.e., skin tone or
face shape), but did differ in comparison to prototypical Black
faces (13). Thus, infants may rely on both facial shape associ-
ated with a racial group and skin tone to distinguish same-
from other-race faces.
Do these examples reflect individuals’ ability to perceptually
differentiate racial categories or merely to differentiate what is
familiar and what is not? As studies often involve comparing
familiar and unfamiliar race faces, this effectively assesses
whether children can separate their familiar group from a per-
ceptually distinct group (11). To build on this work, researchers
should present many groups of unfamiliar other-race faces to
further examine infants’ ability to perceptually differentiate and
categorize faces based on race (cf. 12).
Although it is unclear whether infants’ abilities to categorize
by race reflect more than perceptual differentiation, the central
role of cultural context in these effects deserves emphasis.
Because biases in visual attention are not present at birth (5),
limited exposure to other-race faces may lead to the perceptual
narrowing favoring same-race faces. Indeed, in one study, White
and Black 3-month-olds in Israel who are exposed frequently to
faces from both these racial groups did not look preferentially
toward faces of a same-race relative to other-race faces (6). Even
minimal exposure to other-race faces in infancy facilitates the
ability to recognize other-race faces (14–16). Thus, from a very young age, infants display sensitivity to race that is driven by
cultural context, such as the faces they are exposed to in their
environment.
Toddlers
Recent studies raise questions about the extent to which young
toddlers readily use perceptual cues to categorize new racial
group exemplars, even if they appear to do so as 6-month-olds.
In one study (17), 19-month-old Jewish Israeli toddlers failed to
match new exemplars to a category of exemplars they had just
been familiarized with, including those high in perceptual (e.g.,
gender, race, shirt color) and cultural (e.g., ethnicity) salience,
unless the category exemplars were paired with a novel category
label (e.g., “Look, a Tiroli”) during familiarization. In contrast,
26-month-olds matched new race and gender exemplars with
Child Development Perspectives, Volume 10, Number 1, 2016, Pages 33–38
34 Kristin Pauker, Amanda Williams, and Jennifer R. Steele
the expected category (i.e., selecting a Black target after being
familiarized with color photographs of Black people), regardless
of whether category exemplars were paired with a novel category
label. Thus, younger toddlers’ representation of racial categories
apparently relies on cultural input (e.g., category labels) rather
than emerging solely based on visual cues.
Does being able to perceptually differentiate racial categories
correspond with viewing race as a meaningful, psychologically
salient category that guides behavior (1)? Early in development
it does not, because in infancy, looking preferences are unre-
lated to social behavior. At 10 months, when infants in homoge-
nous cultural contexts robustly recognize same-race compared to
other-race faces, White American infants do not prefer toys
offered by video-recorded White women over those offered by
video-recorded Black women (18). Even older toddlers fail to
demonstrate race-based differences in behavior: White Ameri-
can 2- to 3-year-olds are equally likely to give toys to White or
Black women depicted in color photographs (18). Furthermore,
when the experimental context places social categories in com-
petition, children may prioritize categories other than race and
these may predict behavior (19). When presented simultane-
ously with color photographs of children or adults that vary
systematically by gender and race, White American 3- to 4-
year-olds’ friendship selections, inferences about shared prefer-
ences, allocation and acceptance of toys, and preference for
novel activities and objects are determined more by gender than
race (20, 21).
Children
Children may perceptually differentiate racial group members
based on similar features. But when provided with category
labels, by ages 3 or 4, White Canadian children can identify
the racial group membership of targets depicted in color pho-
tographs (in accordance with adult judgments; 22), and by
ages 6–8, both Black and White children can consistently classify others by race (23). However, in studies of target
groups other than Blacks and Whites, race is not as psycho-
logically salient. For example, when asked to sort color pho-
tographs of children by racial label (White, Black, Asian),
only a slim majority (60%) of White, Black, and Asian 3- to
5-year-olds from multiracial schools in the United Kingdom
used the terms in a manner consistent with adult categoriza-
tions (24). Additionally, when studies included a wider range
of stimuli, such as computer-generated faces that varied in
their prototypicality (in both skin tone and physiognomy), pre-
dominantly White American 4- to 9-year-olds relied more on
skin color than physiognomy when categorizing by race (25;
see also 26). Children did not use facial features as category-
diagnostic information in the same way as adults do, suggest-
ing that children may not have an adult-like conceptualization
of race. These results raise the possibility that past findings
may depend primarily on children’s directed attention to cate-
gory labels and skin color.
LOOKING FORWARD: BRINGING CONTEXT
INTO FOCUS
Although we know much about when children can categorize by
race, we do not know a great deal about when they do so sponta-
neously and what factors affect these categorizations. Further-
more, how much of our conclusion—that race is perceptually discernible by 3 months and explicitly identifiable around
6 years—is based on the stability or homogeneity of the tasks, groups, or environments in studies? In other words, are the con-
clusions about the development of racial categorization biased
by the experimental and cultural contexts in which researchers
have asked these questions? We believe they may be.
As an illustration, we used an open-ended measure to capture
how 8- to 12-year-olds in the continental United States and
Hawaii categorized prototypical White and Black target chil-
dren, depicted in color photographs, by race (27). While White,
Asian, and Latino monoracial and multiracial children in the
continental United States typically listed one racial label per
target, consistent with adult categorizations (e.g., they labeled
the Black target as African American), in Hawaii, White, Asian,
and Black monoracial and multiracial children tended to per-
ceive the monoracial targets as multiracial or belonging to many
groups. Both White and Black targets were described on average
by 3–4 racial/ethnic labels (e.g., labeling the Black target as Black, Chinese, and Native Hawaiian). Perhaps because of their
experience with a large multiracial population (23% of Hawaii
residents identify as multiracial), children growing up in Hawaii
may default to a multiracial prototype and be less likely to rely
on perceptual cues to categorize racially because they are less
predictive in this environment. This example illustrates how
expanding our methods (e.g., moving beyond forced choice or
labels provided by the experimenter) and highlighting where
research is conducted (e.g., a heterogeneous, highly multiracial
environment) can provide new insights into racial categorization.
Although such less structured tasks are not without limits (e.g.,
reliance on children’s verbal abilities, difficulties in scoring
responses), results from these measures can clarify how we
interpret responses on more structured tasks that assess chil-
dren’s racial categorization and ensuing attitudes. Researchers
should look carefully at how experimental and cultural contexts
affect our understanding of racial categorization across develop-
ment. Specifically, we need to consider how we ask the ques-
tions (i.e., our methods and stimuli), where we ask them (i.e.,
the diversity of the child’s surrounding environment), and whom
we ask (i.e., the diversity of the groups we study).
Methods and Stimuli
Many of the tasks used to examine racial categorization inadver-
tently increase the salience of race in the experiment by, for
example, explicitly using racial labels, using racially prototypi-
cal targets, or making comparisons that differ only by race and
not by other competing social categories (e.g., gender, age). In
Child Development Perspectives, Volume 10, Number 1, 2016, Pages 33–38
Racial Categorization in Context 35
open-ended spontaneous description tasks (e.g., a child sees a
target and is prompted, “Tell me about this person; what do you
see?”), White, Black, and Asian preschool and elementary
school children in monoracial and multiracial cultures mention
race rarely (24, 28, 29). However, when children are asked to
sort photos that vary by dimensions (e.g., race, gender, facial
expression, age, clothing) into piles that “go together,” children’s
use of race as a spontaneous sorting dimension increases with
age (24, 30), becoming more reliable around 6 years (30). How
racial categorization is assessed can therefore lead to differing
conclusions about the extent to which children spontaneously
categorize others by race.
Attending to whether the experimental context makes race
psychologically salient does not inherently value unstructured
over structured tasks. Rather, it should help us expand our
repertoire of experimental tasks, interpret more effectively
results that vary across experimental context, and provide fur-
ther insight into the conditions under which others will be spon-
taneously or deliberately categorized by race. For example,
attention to experimental context may affect the interpretation of
valuable, highly structured measures, such as those that assess
children’s implicit racial biases. In tasks where targets are cate-
gorized by race (i.e., the Implicit Association Test), White
American participants display an implicit pro-White (relative to
Black) bias at 6 years that remains stable into adulthood (31).
But measures that do not require overt racial categorization (i.e.,
the Affective Priming Task) yield a different developmental tra-
jectory: Among White German 9- to 15-year-olds, implicit bias
(in the form of out-group negativity) emerged only in early ado-
lescence (32; see also 33). Thus, even among implicit measures,
racial salience in the experimental context may affect research-
ers’ conclusions. Experimental contexts that increase the sal-
ience of racial categories may overestimate the extent to which
children use race spontaneously when perceiving other people.
Similarly, the focus on prototypical exemplars of various racial
groups may artificially heighten children’s attention to race. Not
only does this drastically oversimplify the task children face
when they meet a new person, but also the representation of
stimuli in most experiments reduces within-race variation and
underestimates the dynamic nature of how we perceive other
people (34). We must broaden the range of stimuli used to
include racially ambiguous and multiracial targets to deepen
our understanding of the categorization process (35–37). Similar to adults, primarily majority (i.e., White American) children are
flexible in how they categorize racially ambiguous faces, inte-
grating both visual and top-down category cues (38), or using
their intuitive understanding of race as distinct and immutable
(i.e., essentialist reasoning) to guide how they process and
remember racially ambiguous faces (39). Examining racially
ambiguous and multiracial targets can facilitate our understand-
ing of how conceptual knowledge may bias the category judg-
ments of perceptually identical stimuli. Researchers should also
examine the extent to which different social categories (e.g., race
and gender) intersect to inform perception and social categoriza-
tion (40). Finally, studies have begun to rely on more implicit
measures of spontaneous categorization (33, 41, 42), which is an
important area to develop.
Diversity of Cultural Contexts and Populations
As a whole, most research on racial categorization has been con-
ducted in relatively homogenous cultural contexts (often in the
United States), primarily with White children. Although we have
cited research from several countries (e.g., Canada, China,
Ethiopia, Israel, the United Kingdom, the United States),
researchers must examine both racially homogeneous and
heterogeneous cultural contexts and groups. We need to include
more racial-minority children in this work, including multiracial
children who have been almost entirely excluded (cf. 4, 43). In
studies that explicitly examined more heterogeneous cultural
contexts, where children have exposure to people from a variety
of racial groups, diversity can allow children to maintain greater
flexibility in components of racial categorization. For example,
in one study, infants with intensive cross-race experience did
not look preferentially toward same-race faces (6), and in
another study, older children in a more diverse city were less
likely than children in a rural community to view race as a natu-
ral kind (44). In addition, even within the same cultural context,
children from a minority group (e.g., Black) may categorize
others by race more readily (24, 45), and integrate perceptual
and conceptual knowledge about race earlier to inform category
judgments (36).
CONCLUSION
In this article, we reviewed studies on racial categorization in
childhood and put their findings in context by highlighting that
how, where, and to whom we ask our research questions can
influence our conclusions. While race is perceptually discrim-
inable early in infancy and used spontaneously by children as
young as 6 years to sort others, racial categorization depends on
the immediate (experimental) and broader (cultural) context. To
deepen our knowledge of the conditions under which children
consistently and spontaneously categorize others by race, we
must deepen our understanding of how context can influence
the cues that children attend to when categorizing others.
REFERENCES
1. Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2007). Developmental intergroup the- ory: Explaining and reducing children’s social stereotyping and prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 162–166. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00496.x
2. Rhodes, M. (2013). How two intuitive theories shape the develop- ment of social categorization. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 12– 16. doi:10.1111/cdep.12007
3. Pauker, K., Williams, A., & Steele, J. R. (in press). The develop- ment of racial categorization in childhood. In A. Rutland, D.
Child Development Perspectives, Volume 10, Number 1, 2016, Pages 33–38
36 Kristin Pauker, Amanda Williams, and Jennifer R. Steele
Nesdale & C. S. Brown (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of group processes in children and adolescents. New York, NY: Wiley.
4. Pauker, K., Xu, Y., Williams, A., & Biddle, A. M. (in press). Race essentialism and social contextual differences in children’s racial stereotyping. Child Development. Advance online publication.
5. Kelly, D. J., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., Gibson, A., Smith, M., . . . Pascalis, O. (2005). Three-month-olds, but not newborns, prefer own-race faces. Developmental Science, 8, F31–F36. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.20050434a.x
6. Bar-Haim, Y., Ziv, T., Lamy, D., & Hodes, R. M. (2006). Nature and nurture in own-race face processing. Psychological Science, 17, 159–163. doi:10.1111/j.1467-280.2006.01679.x
7. Kelly, D. J., Liu, S., Ge, L., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., . . . Pascalis, O. (2007). Cross-race preferences for same-race faces extend beyond the African versus Caucasian contrast in 3-month- old infants. Infancy, 11, 87–95. doi:10.1207/s15327078in1101_4
8. Hugenberg, K., Young, S. G., Bernstein, M. J., & Sacco, D. (2010). The categorization-individuation model: An integrative account of the other-race recognition deficit. Psychological Review, 117, 1168– 1187. doi:10.1037/a0020463
9. Kelly, D. J., Liu, S., Lee, K., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., & Ge, L. (2009). Development of the other-race effect during infancy: Evidence toward universality? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 105–114. doi:10.1016/j.jecp/2009.01.006
10. Kelly, D. J., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., Ge, L., & Pascalis, O. (2007). The other-race effect develops during infancy: Evidence of perceptual narrowing. Psychological Science, 18, 1084–1089. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02029.x
11. Anzures, G., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., & Lee, K. (2010). Categorization, categorical perception, and asymmetry in infants’ representation of face race. Developmental Science, 13, 553– 564. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00900x
12. Quinn, P. C., Lee, K., Pascalis, O., & Tanaka, J. W. (2015). Narrow- ing in categorical responding to other-race face classes by infants. Developmental Science, 1–10. Advance online publication. doi:10.1111/desc.12301
13. Balas, B., Westerlund, A., Hung, K., & Nelson, C. A. (2011). Shape, color and the other-race effect in the infant brain. Developmental Science, 14, 892–900. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01039.x
14. Anzures, G., Wheeler, A., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., Heron-Delaney, M., . . . Lee, K. (2012). Brief daily exposures to Asian females reverses perceptual narrowing for Asian faces in Cau- casian infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 484–495. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.04.005
15. Heron-Delaney, M., Anzures, G., Herbert, J. S., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Tanaka, J. W., . . . Pascalis, O. (2011). Perceptual training prevents the emergence of the other race effect during infancy. PLoS ONE, 6, e19858. doi:10.1371/journal/pone.0019858
16. Spangler, S. M., Schwarzer, G., Freitag, C., Vierhaus, M., Teubert, M., Fassbender, I., . . . Keller, H. (2013). The other-race effect in a longitudinal sample of 3-, 6- and 9-month-old infants: Evidence of a training effect. Infancy, 18, 516–533. doi:10.1111/j.1532- 7078.2012.00137.x
17. Diesendruck, G., & Deblinger-Tangi, R. (2014). The linguistic con- struction of social categories in toddlers. Child Development, 85, 114–123. doi:10.1111/cdev.12130
18. Kinzler, K. D., & Spelke, E. S. (2011). Do infants show social pref- erences for people differing in race? Cognition, 119, 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.10.019
19. Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., & Correll, J. (2010). Priorities in social categories. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 581– 592. doi:10.1002/ejsp.739
20. Shutts, K., Banaji, M. R., & Spelke, E. S. (2010). Social categories guide young children’s preferences for novel objects. Developmental Science, 13, 599–610. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00913.x
21. Shutts, K., Roben, C. K. P., & Spelke, E. S. (2013). Children’s use of social categories in thinking about people and social relation- ships. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14, 35–62. doi:10.1080/15248372.2011.638686
22. Aboud, F. E. (2003). The formation of ingroup favoritism and out- group prejudice in young children: Are they distinct attitudes? Developmental Psychology, 39, 48–60. doi:10.1037/0012- 1649.39.1.48
23. Aboud, F. E. (1988). Children and prejudice. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell.
24. Lam, V., Guerrero, S., Damree, N., & Enesco, I. (2011). Young chil- dren’s racial awareness and affect and their perceptions about moth- ers’ racial affect in a multiracial context. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 842–864. doi:10.1348/2044- 835.002013
25. Dunham, Y., Stepanova, E. V., Dotsch, R., & Todorov, A. (2015). The development of race-based perceptual categorization: Skin color dominates early category judgments. Developmental Science, 18, 469–483. doi:10.1111/desc.12228
26. Balas, B., Peissig, J., & Moulson, M. (2015). Children (but not adults) judge similarity in own- and other-race faces by the color of their skin. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 56–66. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.09.009
27. Chong, A., Coffinet, L., Tagomori, J., Meyers, C., Carpinella, C., & Pauker, K. (2015, March). What are you? Differences in racial label- ing between Hawai’i and Northern California children. Poster pre- sented at the Biennial Meeting of Society for Research in Child Development, Philadelphia, PA.
28. Ramsey, P. G. (1991). The salience of race in young children grow- ing up in an all-White community. Journal of Educational Psychol- ogy, 83, 28–34. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.83.1.28
29. Semaj, L. (1980). The development of racial evaluation and prefer- ence: A cognitive approach. Journal of Black Psychology, 6, 59–79. doi:10.1177/009579848000600201
30. Pauker, K., Ambady, N., & Apfelbaum, E. P. (2010). Race salience and essentialist thinking in racial stereotype development. Child Development, 81, 1799–1813. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.0 1511.x
31. Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). The development of implicit attitudes: Evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and adulthood. Psychological Science, 17, 53–58. doi:10.1111/j.1467- 9280.2005.01664.x
32. Degner, J., & Wentura, D. (2010). Automatic activation of prejudice in children and adolescents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 356–374. doi:10.1037/a0017993
33. Williams, A., & Steele, J. R. (2015). It’s not you, it’s me: The devel- opment of implicit racial attitudes in childhood. Unpublished manu- script.
34. Freeman, J. B., & Ambady, N. (2011). A dynamic interactive theory of person construal. Psychological Review, 118, 247–279. doi:10.1037/10022327
35. Dunham, Y., Chen, E. E., & Banaji, M. R. (2013). Two signatures of implicit intergroup attitudes: Developmental invariance and early
Child Development Perspectives, Volume 10, Number 1, 2016, Pages 33–38
Racial Categorization in Context 37
enculturation. Psychological Science, 24, 860–868. doi:10.1177/ 0956797612463081
36. Roberts, S. O., & Gelman, S. A. (2015). Do children see in Black and White? Children’s and adults’ categorizations of multiracial individuals. Child Development. Advance online publication. doi:10.1111/cdev.12410
37. Xiao, W. S., Fu, G., Quinn, P. C., Quinn, J., Tanaka, J. W., Pascalis, O., & Lee, K. (2015). Individuation training with other-race faces reduces preschoolers’ implicit racial bias: A link between percep- tual and social representation of faces in children. Developmental Science, 18, 655–663. doi:10.1111/desc.12241
38. Shutts, K., & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). An ambiguous-race illusion in children’s face memory. Psychological Science, 18, 763–767. doi:10.1111/j/1467-9280.2007.01975.x
39. Gaither, S. E., Schultz, J. R., Pauker, K., Sommers, S. R., Maddox, K. B., & Ambady, N. (2014). Essentialist thinking predicts decre- ments in children’s memory for racially-ambiguous faces. Develop- mental Psychology, 50, 482–488. doi:10.1037/a0033493
40. Johnson, K. L., Freeman, J. B., & Pauker, K. (2012). Race is gendered: How covarying phenotypes and stereotypes bias sex
categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 116–131. doi:10.1037/a0025335
41. Bennett, M., & Sani, F. (2003). The role of target gender and race in children’s encoding of category-neutral person information. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 99–112. doi:10.1348/ 026151003321164645
42. Weisman, K., Johnson, M. V., & Shutts, K. (2014). Young children’s automatic encoding of social categories. Developmental Science. Advance online publication. doi:10.1111/desc.12269
43. Gaither, S. E., Pauker, K., & Johnson, S. (2012). Biracial and mono- racial infant own-race face perception: An eye-tracking study. Developmental Science, 15, 775–782. doi:10.1037/a0033493
44. Rhodes, M., & Gelman, S. A. (2009). Five-year-olds’ beliefs about the discreteness of category boundaries for animals and artifacts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 920–924. doi:10.3758/ PBR.16.5.920
45. Kinzler, K. D., & Dautel, J. B. (2012). Children’s essentialist reason- ing about language and race. Developmental Science, 15, 131–138. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.011101.x
Child Development Perspectives, Volume 10, Number 1, 2016, Pages 33–38
38 Kristin Pauker, Amanda Williams, and Jennifer R. Steele

Get help from top-rated tutors in any subject.
Efficiently complete your homework and academic assignments by getting help from the experts at homeworkarchive.com