SOCIAL MEDIA 1
Social Media
Jesse Fox, Ohio State University, USA
Bree McEwan, DePaul University, USA
Citation:
Fox, J., & McEwan, B. (2019). Social media. In M. B. Oliver, A. Raney, & J. Bryant (Eds.),
Media effects: Advances in theory and research (4th ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.
Order the book here: https://www.crcpress.com/Media-Effects-Advances-in-Theory-and-
Research/Oliver-Raney-Bryant/p/book/9781138590229
Introduction
Although the term social media was not commonly adopted until the 2000s,
masspersonal communication channels have existed since the advent of networked digital
communication. Many outlets have emerged for people to interact online, including bulletin
board systems (BBSs), newsgroups, multi-user dungeons (MUDs), social networking sites, and
massively multiplayer online games.
Given the impossibility of representing the entire scope of social media research in one
chapter, we focus largely on research representing notable distinctions from other computer-
mediated communication (CMC) research due to affordances. Affordances are inherent
functional attributes of an object that emerge when a user interacts with it (Gibson, 1979).
Recent work has specified important social media affordances (Rice et al., 2017; Treem &
Leonardi, 2013) and clarified differences in perceptions of affordances across social media and
other channels (Fox & McEwan, 2017).
SOCIAL MEDIA 2
This review will first discuss conceptualizing social media. Next, we identify notable
affordances and elaborate popular types and uses of social media. Then we review focal research
areas. After identifying limitations of extant research and providing goals for scholarship, we
conclude by considering the future of social media.
Conceptualizing Social Media
One of the greatest challenges in reviewing research on social media is establishing scope
and boundary conditions, given a long history of loose conceptualization. As Web 2.0
development exploded, companies and popular media were quick to label any internet-based
technologies facilitating interpersonal interaction as “social media.” Although such a broad
conceptualization offered limited utility to scholars, definitions emerging in the concurrent
research literature were rarely any clearer (see Carr & Hayes, 2015, for a review).
One issue is scholars have often derived conceptualizations from currently popular
technologies (Carr & Hayes, 2015). Further, they may be based on dominant user practices
reflecting a relatively homogeneous group of users, particularly in early phases of a technology’s
diffusion and adoption. One example is the concept of social networking sites (SNSs), a subset
of social media defined by Donath and boyd (2004) as “online environments in which people
create a self-descriptive profile and then make links to other people they know on the site,
creating a network of personal connections” (p. 12). boyd and Ellison (2007) later rejected the
term “social networking site” in favor of “social network site,” based on data indicating people’s
Facebook networks reflected existing offline networks and forging new relationships was not a
primary goal (Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007). Beer (2008) cautioned against rebranding, but
scholars widely adopted the modified term even though contemporaneous research found SNSs
were commonly used to expand networks (e.g., Grasmuck, Martin, & Zhao, 2009).
SOCIAL MEDIA 3
As this example illustrates, technology researchers face an ongoing struggle: Sound
scholarship demands consistent, clear, and valid conceptualizations, yet scholars examining
rapidly evolving technologies must aim at blurry, constantly moving targets. Scholars are not
fortune tellers, yet they must attempt to both encompass the current state of a technology and
predict how it may evolve in the future. Otherwise, conceptualizations constantly shift and
related theorizing is tenuous at best; irrelevance and obsolescence is a perpetual threat.
Carr and Hayes (2015) made an admirable attempt to synthesize definitions and establish
parameters, defining social media as “internet-based, disentrained, and persistent channels of
masspersonal communication facilitating perceptions of interactions among users, deriving value
primarily from user-generated content” (p. 49). They further clarified social media “allow users
to opportunistically interact and selectively self-present, either in real-time or asynchronously”
(p. 50). This definition presents some strengths in conceptualization, such as defining social
media as masspersonal, meaning they have the capacity to deliver personalized messages to a
broad audience (O’Sullivan & Carr, 2018). Other elements of this definition present some
unclear constraints such as how the value of user-generated content is established and whether
social media require interactions between human users.
Given the ambiguity, we have not adopted a specific definition for this review. Rather,
we adopt an affordance-based approach to delineate what will be considered social media.
Although Carr and Hayes (2015) critiqued affordance-based conceptualizations of social media,
the definitions they provide explicitly cite affordances (synchronicity and channel persistence,
related to accessibility) and imply others (disentrainment indicates conversational control,
whereas selective self-presentation suggests editability). Further, a recent conceptualization of
masspersonal communication is founded on affordances (O’Sullivan & Carr, 2018).
SOCIAL MEDIA 4
Universal Affordances of Social Media
Specific affordances are required to differentiate social media from other channels:
interactivity, accessibility, visibility, and personalization. Social media provide at least two
layers of interactivity: interacting with a responsive mediated system and interacting socially
with other users (Sundar, 2007). Users must be able to manipulate a responsive interface and
generate some form of socially relevant content. Further, this system must facilitate interactivity
among users through some modality, such as exchanging text-based messages or images,
speaking through an audio channel, or communicating via avatars. Users may also generate
system cues, such as page views or aggregated “likes,” that communicate information to other
users (Walther & Jang, 2012). Thus, social media often provide multiple ways of achieving
social interactivity through both user-generated and system-generated content.
Another key affordance is accessibility, or the capability of using a channel regardless of
time, place, structural limitations, or other constraints (Culnan & Markus, 1987; Fox & McEwan,
2017). Social media maintain a certain level of accessibility due to their continual availability, as
sites continue to function regardless of any individual user’s participation (Carr & Hayes, 2015).
Digital divides based on factors such as age, socioeconomic status, and education still exist in
regards to internet access and social media adoption (e.g., Hargittai, in press).
Given their masspersonal nature, social media must also enable visibility of social
interactions to a wide audience (Treem & Leonardi, 2013), which may or may not align with the
imagined audience users anticipate when posting (Marwick & boyd, 2011). Many platforms offer
privacy settings to allow users some control over who can see their content. Because digital
content is transmitted and replicated easily, however, a message can be spread through shares,
reposts, and retweets to a wide, sometimes unintended, audience with little effort. Because of this
SOCIAL MEDIA 5
scalability, messages can become viral, meaning they are widely dispersed by users across vast
audiences, sometimes reaching millions of users almost instantly (Marwick & Lewis, 2017).
Despite the potentially broad audience, another requisite affordance of social media as a
masspersonal channel is that users can personalize messages (O’Sullivan & Carr, 2018). Users
can tailor their message or direct it to a smaller segment of the audience.
Aside from these universal affordances, other affordances vary considerably across social
media and are used to distinguish different types or explain discrepancies in uses or effects.
Articulating these affordances allows scholars to perform a nuanced evaluation of social media,
distinguish them from other communication channels, and inform theorizing.
Other Social Media Affordances
Perhaps the most commonly examined affordance is anonymity or identifiability, the
degree to which users’ real names or true identities can be concealed in a channel (Lea & Spears,
1991). Anonymity has been central to theorizing on online disinhibition, in which people engage
in less socially normative behavior online (e.g., social identity model of deindividuation effects,
Lea & Spears, 1991). Even if users try to maintain anonymity, they often leak cues to their
identity through profile information, network members, geolocated content, or “likes” (see Shu,
Wang, Tang, Zafarani, & Liu, 2017, for a review).
Some affordances shape how interactions transpire via social media. Sites vary in
synchronicity, or the timing of message exchange (Culnan & Markus, 1987). Conversational
control involves managing the mechanics of an interaction, such as regulating turn-taking or
ending a dialogue (Fox & McEwan, 2017). For example, a system may limit how many times a
user can respond to a post, or users may be able to “mute” or “block” others’ comments.
SOCIAL MEDIA 6
Other affordances determine the nature of messages and content. Bandwidth is the scope
of verbal and nonverbal cues that can be conveyed through various modalities, such as text,
graphics, audio, or video (Walther & Parks, 2002). Limited bandwidth and asynchronicity
facilitate editability, or the capacity to revise messages (Walther, 1996). The digital materiality
of social media messages affords easy modification. This materiality also makes messages easily
replicated and transmitted, facilitating the persistence of messages (Treem & Leonardi, 2013).
Although users may be able to delete messages, these can also be shared, downloaded, or stored,
and so may remain long after the original transmission.
Finally, network association enables users to visibly link to other users, creating a
traceable network of connections. Through common nodes or “friends,” users can identify other
network members and often access their content (boyd & Ellison, 2007; Treem & Leonardi,
2013), creating networks of strong, weak, and latent ties (Haythornthwaite, 2005). Network
association facilitates context collapse, which is experienced when social circles normally
maintained separately (such as a person’s co-workers, their former classmates, and their
extended family) are blended in a shared environment (Marwick & boyd, 2011).
Types and Uses of Social Media
Several types of social media have emerged, although individual platforms or groups may
fall into multiple categories. The primary functions of social media are interacting with others
and sharing information; other uses and gratifications vary across sites (McEwan, 2015). Some
of the earliest social media research focused on bulletin board systems, online forums, and online
communities (e.g., Jones, 1995; Rheingold, 1993; Wellman & Hampton, 1999). Discussion
forums may be organized around topics (e.g., Reddit, 4chan) or specific content (e.g., comment
sections on news articles). Some forums may be considered online communities, which are
SOCIAL MEDIA 7
characterized by aggregates of individuals who share a common feature or interest (McEwan,
2015). Online community members engage in social goals such as forming relationships,
building a collective culture, and creating, negotiating, and sharing group norms (Baym, 2010).
Among the most common social media are social networking sites (SNSs) such as
Facebook, Twitter, Instagram, and LinkedIn, preceded by sites such as Friendster, MySpace,
Hyves, and Orkut. SNSs are characterized by the abilities to create a profile, link to other users,
and observe how other users are connected within the broader network sustain a profile via the
affordance of network association (boyd & Ellison, 2007). Thus, establishing social connections,
maintaining relationships, and building networks are common uses of SNSs.
Some social media are characterized by richer environments and tasks beyond socializing
and exchanging information. Social virtual worlds (e.g., Second Life and VRChat), open
sandbox platforms (e.g., Roblox), and massively multiplayer online games (e.g., Fortnite)
typically enable richer forms of self-representation such as customizable avatars (Yee, 2014) and
near synchronous communication via text chat, voice chat, or avatar interactions. Given their
diversity of tasks, other common uses for these sites include collaboration, competition,
achievement, discovery, role-playing, and escapism (Yee, 2014).
Content-based social media sites often constitute participatory cultures, as these sites are
designed primarily to share, consume, and interact with material created or curated by the user
(e.g., Pinterest, Tumblr, YouTube, Twitch, blogging communities, wikis; Jenkins, 2006). Other
types of social media are distinguished by affordances, such as locative social media designed to
track and report users’ movements to other users (e.g., FourSquare, Dodgeball; Humphreys,
2007) and anonymous sites designed to elicit disinhibited disclosures (e.g., Whisper, Yik Yak,
Formspring). Other types of social media are likely to emerge with future technologies.
SOCIAL MEDIA 8
Areas of Social Media Research and Theorizing
Social media can influence human communication processes in three ways. First, social
media may simply accommodate the manifestation of communication phenomena without
changing the communication process. Existing theories do not require additional clarification
because the channel does not have a notable impact. For example, Carpenter and Spottswood
(2013) observed that self-expansion effects previously studied in offline interactions between
romantic partners can also occur via Facebook. Second, social media may amplify observed
communication processes by expediting, expanding, or enhancing phenomena compared to other
channels. For example, social media allow activists to organize collective action more efficiently
than other channels (Tufecki, 2017). Regarding amplification, existing theories may increase
their explanatory power by assessing relevant social media affordances, but they do not require
major revisions. Finally, social media may alter communication processes in fundamentally
different ways. For example, according to social penetration theory, if two strangers meet face-
to-face, they would have to engage in ongoing self-disclosure to get to know each other. On an
SNS like Facebook, however, they may have access to considerable information about each other
in terms of breadth (tastes, friends, opinions) and possibly depth, perhaps without ever
interacting. When social media alter processes, theories must be revised or new theories must be
developed. In this review, we will largely focus on research examining amplification and
alteration via social media.
Self-Presentation, Impression Management, and Identity Performance
Social media have altered how identities are performed online, but sites are not
equivalent in their effects on identity performance. Affordances like asynchronicity and
editability enable users to engage in selective self-presentation and more diligent impression
SOCIAL MEDIA 9
management (Walther, 1996). Anonymous spaces can grant more flexibility in the identities
people can present (McEwan, 2015; Turkle, 1984). These affordances are valuable for people
who experience marginalization offline (McKenna & Bargh, 1999). Yet, network association can
complicate identity performance; due to context collapse, members of one social audience
segment may see identity performances meant for another audience (Marwick & boyd, 2011).
McEwan (2015) proposed considering social media spaces along a fixed-flexible
continuum contingent on the access of particular types of network members. Fixedness or
flexibility is based on varying degrees of corporality, anonymity, and persistence. Flexible
spaces are divorced from stable connections between network members, including a separation
from one’s online communication and a corporeal self (i.e., unanchored relationships). Within
these spaces participants can engage in identity experimentation (e.g., Livingstone, 2008;
Valkenberg, Schouten, & Peter, 2005) or even present as multiple identities (Turkle, 1984).
Within fixed network spaces, users’ performed identities must be coherent and consistent for at
least one potential social audience and often for multiple audience contexts (e.g., Stutzman,
Gross, & Acquisiti, 2013). People may manage fixidity by maintaining different profiles with
different audiences. Alternatively, they may attempt to present an identity appropriate for
multiple audience segments (Hogan, 2010).
Fixidity is also influenced by visible feedback offered by other users. According to
warranting theory, because online self-presentation can be more easily manipulated by the
source, users seek out and evaluate cues to assess the veracity of this self-presentation (Walther
& Parks, 2002). The less a cue can be manipulated by the source, the more value and weight it
has to the user (DeAndrea, 2014). On social media, people trust friends’ comments about a
SOCIAL MEDIA 10
person more than the person’s self-presentation (Walther, Van Der Heide, Hamel, & Shulman,
2009). In this way, identity performance is constrained by affordances and audience members.
Disclosure and Privacy
Self-disclosure and privacy have been a popular focus for social media research
(Stoycheff, Liu, Wiwobo, & Nanni, 2017). Several concepts have been clarified through research
focused on social media, such as context collapse, the imagined audience, and the privacy
paradox, which describes the inconsistency between users’ privacy concerns and their behaviors
(Barnes, 2006; Marwick & boyd, 2011). The masspersonal nature of social media further
complicates privacy management and what is perceived as acceptable self-disclosure.
To examine this, Bazarova (2012) adopted a disclosure personalism framework to
examine perceptions of intimate and nonintimate messages shared on Facebook. Comparing
masspersonal, public Facebook posts and private interpersonal Facebook messaging, Bazarova
found people judged senders and their intimate disclosures more negatively in masspersonal
contexts. Examining senders, Bazarova and Choi (2014) extended existing theorizing to a
functional model of SNS disclosure, which argues senders’ impression management concerns are
amplified by SNS affordances such as visibility and personalization.
Relationship Development and Maintenance
Within existing relationships, a social media site may become an additional channel
through which to communicate, contributing to media multiplexity (Haythornthwaite, 2005).
Social media may also facilitate new relationships (Parks & Floyd, 1996; Utz, 2000). Some sites
enable people to connect over shared interests or content, which can minimize factors that may
have prevented a relationship from developing otherwise, such as geographical distance or
demographic dissimilarities (Baym, 2010; McKenna & Bargh, 1999; Parks & Floyd, 1996). In
SOCIAL MEDIA 11
addition to social interaction, users may engage in shared tasks (e.g., game activities, content
moderation) to maintain relationships (Baym, 2010; Ledbetter & Kuznekoff, 2012; Yee, 2014).
SNSs have been a major focus for relational maintenance research. SNSs greatly increase
the number of weak ties maintained by users (Donath & boyd, 2004) and can help solidify
emerging relationships (Ellison et al., 2007). The SNS affordance of network association may
also help people maintain not just direct connections, but also connections between those whom
we see connected to our connections (Ellison, Vitak, Gray, & Lampe, 2014).
People can use SNSs to enact traditional maintenance strategies (Bryant & Marmo, 2009)
as well as site-specific strategies (e.g. McEwan, Fletcher, Eden, & Sumner 2014). For example,
messages directed to specific others indicating social contact and relational assurances are
correlated with satisfaction, liking, and commitment in friendships. Yet, messages seeking
comfort from a general audience are negatively related to these outcomes (McEwan, 2013).
Social Capital, Social Resources, and Social Support
Social capital refers to the value imbued within an individual due to their representation
of other, potentially powerful, network connections (Bourdieu, 1986). Bonding social capital is
associated with close network relationships, and bridging social capital is produced through the
accumulation of weaker ties. Maintained social capital can sometimes be converted into
resources. Through building capital-rich networks, social media users may later be able to
request instrumental and social resources from their network connections. Research has shown
SNSs like Facebook can help users develop bridging social capital and attain resources (e.g.,
Ellison et al., 2007; Ellison et al., 2014).
One such resource is social support, which is commonly obtained through SNSs and
online support groups (e.g., Wright, 2000). Informational and emotional support are easily
SOCIAL MEDIA 12
conveyed online, although tangible support can be more difficult (Mikal, Rice, Abeyta, &
DeVilbiss, 2013). Affordances affect support seeking and provision. Accessibility is key, as
users can obtain support any time; further, they may be able to identify support providers they do
not have access to offline, such as other people with a similar condition (Rains, 2018).
Anonymity may facilitate more disclosure and help-seeking, particularly for those with
stigmatized conditions (Mikal et al., 2013). The visibility of others’ supportive messages on
social media leads users to create higher quality support messages (Li & Feng, 2015).
Seeking support and resources through social media may not always be effective. Some
research has indicated people must be actively engaged with network members to have resource
requests fulfilled (Ellison et al., 2014). In some cases, masspersonal support requests may be
judged negatively by weak tie connections (High, Oeldorf-Hirsch, & Bellur, 2014).
Psychological Well-Being and Health Communication
An evergreen topic for researchers has been the effects of social media use on well-being,
which have been associated with individual traits, consumed content, and the nature of use.
Evidence has been found to support both the social compensation hypothesis (“poor-get-richer,”
in which those lacking offline have needs met online) and social enhancement hypothesis (“rich-
get-richer,” in which those who already have support or resources offline also benefit online;
Seabrook, Kern, & Rickard, 2016). Evidence suggests it is the nature of the communication that
matters. Similar to other channels, positive and supportive interactions are associated with
positive well-being, whereas negative interactions and social comparisons are associated with
depression, loneliness, and anxiety (Seabrook et al., 2016). Excessive use (i.e., “addiction”) has
been associated with negative well-being, although more nuanced findings suggest these are
likely co-occurring outcomes attributable to individual differences (Seabrook et al., 2016), and
SOCIAL MEDIA 13
problematic or maladaptive use (e.g., surveillance of one’s ex-partner or disruptive multitasking)
should be examined more specifically (see Caplan, 2018).
Extremely negative interactions, such as cyberbullying and online harassment (e.g.,
trolling) have shown consistent associations with negative outcomes and may be amplified by
online affordances (Fox & Tang, 2017; Tokunaga, 2010). For example, doxing and revenge porn
involve disclosing a target’s private information without their consent (Marwick & Lewis, 2017).
Although these breaches would also be violating offline, the visibility, scalability, persistence,
and searchability of this information online may make these experiences especially pernicious.
Unfortunately, few studies have compared channels or assessed affordances to determine
whether social media are augmenting effects on mental health. Establishing causality can also be
difficult. One notable experiment manipulated interactivity on Facebook, finding passive
browsing, compared to actively communicating with others, caused a drop in affective well-
being over time. A follow-up field study using experience sampling further clarified passive
browsing was associated with greater feelings of envy, possibly due to social comparison with
one’s ties (Verduyn et al., 2015). A controversial experiment conducted by Facebook tested
network effects by manipulating the visibility of positive or negative emotional posts in users’
newsfeeds and analyzing their subsequent posts. Seeing more positive posts increased users’
positive posts and reduced negative posts whereas seeing more negative posts had the opposite
result, indicating an emotional contagion effect (Kramer, Guillory, & Hancock, 2014).
Other health-related research has investigated information seeking, support seeking, and
patient-provider communication through social media (see Rains, 2018, for a review). Despite
the apparent advantages of SNSs for health interventions, randomized controlled trials have
yielded minimal effects, similar to other media (Yang, 2017). One novel use of social media in
SOCIAL MEDIA 14
the health context has been the tracking of outbreaks (e.g., flu, food poisoning) through SNS
posts and networks (see Charles-Smith et al., 2017, for a review).
Information Diffusion and Evaluation
The convergence of established mass media, online news, and interpersonal sources
within and across social media create information flows that can be difficult for the average user
to evaluate. The source of the message can become ambiguous (Flanagin, 2017). Methods of
curation and censorship by sites (e.g., algorithms, filters, content moderators) and users can
further cloud the veracity and comprehensiveness of presented information (Flanagin, 2017).
Regardless of accuracy, the scalability of social media messages enables murky sources or
misinformation to circulate far and wide (Marwick & Lewis, 2017).
For these reasons, the credibility of information shared via social media is often
questioned. Only three percent of U.S. Americans claim to have a lot of trust in information they
find via social media (Smith & Anderson, 2018). Research indicates, however, users are more
likely to trust digital misinformation shared by friends (Garrett, 2011), and friends’ endorsements
on social media affect news selection (Messing & Westwood, 2014). In this way, social media
may have distinct effects compared to misinformation from other online sources, as users may be
more likely to believe and propagate “fake news” when it is shared by a friend.
The differing affordances of social media sites may influence information diffusion.
Online communities may facilitate homogenous spaces where users can share like views that
become reinforced over time (Wojcieszak, 2010), whereas SNSs often facilitate large weak tie
networks which expose users to greater information diversity (Bakshy, Messing, & Adamic,
2015). Although nonanonymous SNSs like Facebook certainly are not free of incivility, political
SOCIAL MEDIA 15
discussions on SNSs may feature more politeness than anonymous spaces like a YouTube
comments section (Halpern & Gibbs, 2013).
Political Communication and Collective Action
Social media research has indicated amplifying effects on political expression and
collective action, particularly given the linkages and rapid diffusion of information among ties on
social networking sites (e.g., Freelon, McIlwain, & Clark, 2016; Kreiss, 2016). The accessibility
of social media may promote greater political engagement (Papacharissi, 2002). SNS users may
feel more comfortable expressing themselves online and may reach more politically diverse
audiences than they would offline due to network association and scalability (Freelon et al.,
2016), which may also facilitate collective action more efficiently and on a greater scale than
traditional channels (Tufecki, 2017). These same affordances, however, may drive a spiral of
silence for marginalized users (Fox & Warber, 2015).
Another concern is that social media provide unprecedented tactics and power to political
operatives and corporations (Vaidhyanathan, 2018). Facebook demonstrated it could influence
voting behavior by manipulating the visibility of voting messages in users’ feeds (Bond et al.,
2012). Sock puppet accounts and bots can be used to disseminate misinformation, astroturf (i.e.,
disguise operatives’ efforts as grassroots movements), or instigate discord or further polarization
(Marwick & Lewis, 2017). A final concern is that the visibility and aggregation of social media
users’ activities generate considerable data that have been used by political parties, corporations,
and bad actors to identify, target, and silence dissidents; perpetuate misinformation and
propaganda; and influence political outcomes (Pearce & Kendzior, 2012; Vaidhyanathan, 2018).
Future Directions for Social Media Research
SOCIAL MEDIA 16
In the relatively short history of social media research, a few consistent issues have
emerged. Here, we address some observed limitations in existing research, provide some goals
for researchers, and consider emerging social media platforms and their affordances.
Improving Social Media Research
Do your homework. First, social media scholars need to begin their investigations by
digging much deeper than recent social media research on their topic within their field. Lacking a
historical grounding, too often social media researchers commit a false novelty error, in which
they assume a phenomenon is unprecedented and attributable to social media. Often these
phenomena are not novel; rather, they illustrate amplifying effects of social media on existing
processes. If researchers do not conduct historical research across different disciplines, they may
assume trolling (social aggression and harassment), ghosting (relationship dissolution), and fake
news (rumors, gossip, and deceptive journalism) are new occurrences or unique to social media.
Recognizing how phenomena manifest outside of social media contexts is not just sound
empirical practice, it also challenges one-sided narratives in which social media are uniformly
heralded as saving humanity or blamed for destroying it.
Be patient. Social media researchers should not be baited by apparent novelty, media
frenzies, or technophilia; proceed diplomatically and assess similarities to and differences from
existing channels. Historical research may reveal patterns in how related technologies have been
perceived, used, adopted, or abandoned, which may give insight on the durability of an emerging
technology (Rogers, 1962). Another reason patience is advised is that technology users and
practices evolve over time. Initial users are likely to differ from other users in significant ways
(Humphreys, 2007; Rogers, 1962), limiting conclusions or theorizing based on early findings.
SOCIAL MEDIA 17
Diversify. A third goal is to expand the populations, topics, and sites researchers study.
Due to growing internet access, social media audiences are diversifying globally. Various
cultural norms, governmental regulation or monitoring, and literacies influence social media use
(e.g., Pearce & Kendzior, 2012). Additionally, studies are often conducted with a single site
(often Facebook) and findings are erroneously generalized to all social media (Stoycheff et al.,
2017). Sites vary in the nature of their users and audiences, the features they offer, and their
affordances (e.g., Fox & McEwan, 2017). Finally, researchers should also seek out nonusers,
examining digital divides, group differences, and selective avoidance (e.g., Hargittai, in press).
Of particular concern is that individuals, particularly those from marginalized groups, may avoid
or quit using technologies due to negative social experiences (e.g., Fox & Tang, 2017).
Adopt an affordance-based approach. Researchers should account for structural and
perceived affordances when studying social media as they are important determinants of site
selection, use, communicative behaviors, and other effects. Clarifying the role of affordances
will help scholars better predict what findings generalize across sites or across channels more
generally, enabling more flexible and durable theorizing (Fox & McEwan, 2017).
Improve measurement. A fifth goal is to better operationalize social media behavior,
which may include updating one’s profile; creating or sharing content; viewing or interacting
with others’ content; or messaging another user in a private channel. Unfortunately, researchers
typically only assess time spent on sites. Many studies’ hypotheses, however, concern the type of
content participants are encountering and how much they are attending to, processing, and
interacting with such content. For example, a study examining the impact of cross-cutting
political discussions on Twitter needs to assess how much users notice, read, and engage with
such discussions, not how much time they spend watching cute chinchilla videos. Given there are
SOCIAL MEDIA 18
considerable limitations to common techniques (e.g., self-report, automated tracking, experience
sampling, data scrapes; see de Vreese & Neijens, 2016), triangulation of methods is advised.
Researchers should also consider how social media fit within media repertoires as well as
within other social experiences (Stoycheff et al., 2017). Processes may be working in
conjunction, perhaps indicating additive effects. For example, women may receive consonant
messages reinforcing the importance of their physical appearance across traditional media, social
media content, and social interactions. In other cases, notable contradictions may emerge. For
example, individuals who cannot discuss their sexual orientation among known ties may express
themselves freely on unanchored social media (e.g., McKenna & Bargh, 1999).
Conduct ethical research. A final advisement is social media researchers must give
ongoing consideration to a number of ethical issues in our ever-evolving domain. The thirst for
easily accessible data—and perhaps envy of corporations’ stockpiles—does not obviate scholars’
ethical obligations to protect participants and maintain public trust in scientific research. Current
research practices beget two ethical questions: Should researchers use participants’ data without
explicit consent? Should they scrape and aggregate this data and share it freely on the internet?
Researchers must concede users’ outrage about Facebook’s many missteps (see
Vaidhyanathan, 2018) indicates many users are not happy about their data being used without
their awareness or approval. Specific to scientific study, Fiesler and Proferes (2018) surveyed a
sample of Twitter users and found over 60% were not aware researchers could use their tweets.
Nearly half indicated they would feel uncomfortable if their Twitter history was used for
research, and 65% said researchers should not be allowed to use tweets without asking
permission from the user. These findings indicate many users object to researchers taking their
data without informing them or obtaining consent.
SOCIAL MEDIA 19
Researchers must also acknowledge that accumulating and aggregating social media data
inherently increases privacy risks to users. The process generates copies, links data in ways that
may be otherwise not easily discernible, and establishes persistence the user has no control over.
Even if a user deletes their account and all their posts, the researcher is now a curator taking
away the user’s “right to be forgotten” (Rosen, 2012). Aggregate datasets posted online are more
identifiable and create greater risk for participants than scholars may realize, given many forms
of social media data are searchable (e.g., the content of a tweet) and thus potentially identifiable
without names attached—not to mention algorithms already exist that are capable of linking data
across anonymous and nonanonymous sites and identifying users (Shu et al., 2017). Moreover,
scholars cannot anticipate how corporations, insurance companies, employers, law enforcement,
governments, or criminals may use this conveniently compiled data or link it to other records.
Perhaps the underlying issue here is the objectification of social media users: they—we—
are now seen as data points, not people. But what we do on social media sites is more than just
data or information or content. What we create and share through social media are our personal
histories, artistic expressions, memories, feelings, conversations, and secrets. Collectively, these
artifacts constitute a digital self that people want to share with others socially, but they may
object to having placed under a microscope. Social media scholars should consider these ethical
issues carefully in the design, conduct, and reporting of their studies and associated data.
Emerging and Evolving Social Media Platforms
Although it is difficult to determine how emerging forms of social media will manifest
and whether they will endure, some recent social media have provided distinct features or
combinations of affordances. In recent years, for example, Twitch has become an immensely
successful global platform by merging video livestreaming and text-based chat. Streamers can
SOCIAL MEDIA 20
create profiles and broadcast live video to an interactive audience. Although research is emerging
on Twitch (e.g., Taylor, 2018), theorizing has yet to tackle the confluence of one-to-many mass
communication (the streamer broadcasting), many-to-many computer-mediated group
communication (text-based chat among audience members), and one-to-one or many-to-one
computer-mediated interpersonal communication (audience members addressing the streamer).
Social virtual environments such as massively multiplayer online video games (MMOs),
3D virtual worlds, and collaborative virtual reality have grown in popularity. They are often
overlooked within the scope of social media, but by many definitions, these environments
qualify and provide distinct affordances. For example, embodiment is often afforded through the
use of a responsive avatar controlled by keypresses or body movements. In fully immersive
virtual environments, users can move their bodies in natural ways and their avatar responds
accordingly. This mapping may have distinct effects for mediated social interaction compared to
communicating through less natural methods, such as keypresses.
Another crucial consideration for social media researchers will be to account for the
growing influence of computer-generated content, algorithms, and agents (i.e., computer
controlled entities, such as bots and nonplayable characters) in social media spheres. Already,
users may find themselves engaging in Turing tests to determine whether they are interacting
with a human or a computer. As agents become more sophisticated, however, it may be more
difficult to identify when they are masquerading as human users. When senders and receivers are
no longer human or predominantly human-controlled, the applicability of existing
communication theories will have to be reconsidered.
In conclusion, social media have become an integral communication channel for many
people, providing a conduit for interaction, but also amplifying and altering communication
SOCIAL MEDIA 21
processes. As social media and their users continue to evolve, researchers face an ongoing
challenge of keeping pace with changing practices and a changing society.
SOCIAL MEDIA 22
References
Bakshy, E., Messing, S., & Adamic, L. A. (2015). Exposure to ideologically diverse news and
opinion on Facebook. Science, 348, 1130-1132. doi:10.1126/science.aaa1160
Barnes, S. B. (2006). A privacy paradox: Social networking in the United States. First Monday,
11(9). doi:10.5210/fm.v11i9.1394
Baym, N. K. (2010). Social connections in the digital age. Malden, MA: Polity Press.
Bazarova, N. N. (2012). Public intimacy: Disclosure interpretation and social judgments on
Facebook. Journal of Communication, 62, 815-832. doi:10.1111/j.1460-
2466.2012.01664.x
Bazarova, N. N., & Choi, Y. H. (2014). Self-disclosure in social media: Extending the
functional approach to disclosure motivations and characteristics on social network
sites. Journal of Communication, 64, 635-657. doi:10.1111/jcom.12106
Beer, D. D. (2008). Social network(ing) sites…revisiting the story so far: A response to danah
boyd & Nicole Ellison. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 13, 516-529.
doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2008.00408.x
Bond, R. M., Fariss, C. J., Jones, J. J., Kramer, A. D., Marlow, C., Settle, J. E., & Fowler, J. H.
(2012). A 61-million-person experiment in social influence and political mobilization.
Nature, 489, 295-298. doi:10.1038/nature11421
Bourdieu, P. (1986). The forms of capital. In J. E. Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of theory and
research for the sociology of education (pp. 46-58). New York, NY: Greenwood.
boyd, d. m., & Ellison, N. B. (2007). Social network sites: Definition, history, and scholarship.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 13, 210-230. doi:10.1111/j/1083-
6101.2007.00393.x
SOCIAL MEDIA 23
Bryant, E. M., & Marmo, J. (2009). Relational maintenance strategies on Facebook. Kentucky
Journal of Communication, 28, 129-150. Retrieved from EBSCOhost (Accession No.
98060803).
Caplan, S. E. (2018). The changing face of problematic Internet use: An interpersonal
approach. New York, NY: Peter Lang.
Carpenter, C. J., & Spottswood, E. L. (2013). Exploring romantic relationships on social
networking sites using the self-expansion model. Computers in Human Behavior, 29,
1531-1537. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.01.02
Carr, C. T., & Hayes, R. A. (2015). Social media: Defining, developing, and divining. Atlantic
Journal of Communication, 23, 46-65. doi:10.1080/15456870.2015.972282
Charles-Smith, L. E., Reynolds, T. L., Cameron, M. A., Conway, M., Lau, E. H., Olsen, J. M.,
... & Corley, C. D. (2015). Using social media for actionable disease surveillance and
outbreak management: A systematic literature review. PloS One, 10(10), e0139701.
Culnan, M. J., & Markus, M. L. (1987). Information technologies. In F. M. Jablin, L. L.
Putnam, K. H. Roberts, & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Handbook of organizational
communication (pp. 420-443). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
de Vreese, C. H., & Neijens, P. (2016). Measuring media exposure in a changing
communications environment. Communication Methods & Measures, 10, 69-80.
doi:10.1080/19312458.2016.1150441
DeAndrea, D. C. (2014). Advancing warranting theory. Communication Theory, 24, 186-204.
doi: 10.1111/comt.12033
Donath, J., & boyd, d. (2004). Public displays of connection. BT Technology Journal, 22(4),
71-82. doi:10.1023/B:BTTJ.0000047585.06264.cc
SOCIAL MEDIA 24
Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Facebook “friends:” Social
capital and college students’ use of online social network sites. Journal of Computer‐
Mediated Communication, 12, 1143-1168. doi: 10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00367.x
Ellison, N. B., Vitak, J., Gray, R., & Lampe, C. (2014). Cultivating social resources on social
network sites: Facebook relationship maintenance behaviors and their role in social
capital processes. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 19, 855-870. doi:
10.1111/jcc4.12078
Fiesler, C., & Proferes, N. (2018). “Participant” perceptions of Twitter research ethics. Social
Media+Society, 4, 1-14. doi:10.1177/2056305118763366
Flanagin, A. J. (2017). Online social influence and the convergence of mass and interpersonal
communication. Human Communication Research, 43, 450-463.
doi:10.1111/hcre.12116
Fox, J., & McEwan, B. (2017). Distinguishing technologies for social interaction: The
Perceived Social Affordances of Communication Channels Scale. Communication
Monographs, 84, 298-318. doi:10.1080/03637751.2017.1332418
Fox, J., & Tang, W. Y. (2017). Women’s experiences with harassment in online video games:
Rumination, organizational responsiveness, withdrawal, and coping strategies. New
Media & Society, 19, 1290-1307. doi:10.1177/1461444816635778
Fox, J., & Warber, K. M. (2015). Queer identity management and political self-expression on
social networking sites: A co-cultural approach to the spiral of silence. Journal of
Communication, 65, 79-100. doi:10.1111/jcom.12137
SOCIAL MEDIA 25
Freelon, D., McIlwain, C. D., & Clark, M. D. (2016). Beyond the hashtags: #Ferguson,
#Blacklivesmatter, and the online struggle for offline justice. Washington, DC: American
University Center for Media & Social Impact.
Garrett, R. K. (2011). Troubling consequences of online political rumoring. Human
Communication Research, 37, 255-274. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2010.01401.x
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghlin Mifflin.
Grasmuck, S., Martin, J., & Zhao, S. (2009). Ethno-racial identity displays on Facebook.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 15, 158-188. doi:10.1111/j.1083-
6101.2009.01498.x
Halpern, D., & Gibbs, J. (2013). Social media as a catalyst for online deliberation? Exploring
the affordances of Facebook and YouTube for political expression. Computers in
Human Behavior, 29, 1159-1168. doi: 10.1016/j/chb.2012.10.008
Hargittai, E. (in press). Potential biases in big data: Omitted voices on social media. Social
Science Computer Review. doi: 10.1177/0894439318788322
Haythornthwaite, C. (2005). Social networks and Internet connectivity effects. Information,
Community & Society, 8, 125-147. doi: 10.1080/13691180500146185
High, A. C., Oeldorf-Hirsch, A., & Bellur, S. (2014). Misery rarely gets company: The -
influence of emotional bandwidth on supportive communication on Facebook.
Computers in Human Behavior, 34, 79-88. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.01.037
Hogan, B. (2010). The presentation of self in the age of social media: Distinguishing
performances and exhibitions online. Bulletin of Science, Technology & Society, 30,
377-386. doi:10.1177/0270467610385893
SOCIAL MEDIA 26
Humphreys, L. (2007). Mobile social networks and social practice: A case study of Dodgeball.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 13, 341-360. doi:10.1111/j.1083-
6101.2007.00399.x
Jenkins, H. (2006). Fans, bloggers, and gamers: Exploring participatory culture. New York,
NY: NYU Press.
Jones, S. G. (1995). CyberSociety: Computer-mediated communication and community.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Knobloch-Westerwick, S., Sharma, N., Hansen, D. L., & Alter, S. (2005). Impact of popularity
indications on readers’ selective exposure to online news. Journal of Broadcasting &
Electronic Media, 49, 296-313. doi:10.1207/s15506878jobem4903_3
Kramer, A. D., Guillory, J. E., & Hancock, J. T. (2014). Experimental evidence of massive-
scale emotional contagion through social networks. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 111, 8788-8790. doi:10.1073/pnas.1320040111
Kreiss, D. (2016). Prototype politics: Technology-intensive campaigning and the data of
democracy. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Lea, M., & Spears, R. (1991). Computer-mediated communication, deindividuation, and group
decision-making. International Journal of Man Machine Studies, 34, 283-301.
doi:10.1016/0020-7373(91)90045-9
Ledbetter, A. M., & Kuznekoff, J. H. (2012). More than a game: Friendship relational
maintenance and attitudes toward Xbox LIVE Communication. Communication
Research, 39, 269-290. doi: 10.1177/0093650210397042
SOCIAL MEDIA 27
Li, S., & Feng, B. (2015). What to say to an online support-seeker? The influence of others’
responses and support-seekers’ replies. Human Communication Research, 41, 303-326.
doi:10.1111/hcre.12055
Livingstone, S. (2008). Taking risky opportunities in youthful content creation: Teenagers’ use
of social networking sites for intimacy, privacy and self-expression. New Media &
Society, 10, 393-411. doi:10.1177/1461444808089415
Marwick, A. E., & boyd, d. (2011). I tweet honestly, I tweet passionately: Twitter users,
context collapse, and the imagined audience. New Media & Society, 13, 114-133.
doi:10.1177/1461444810365313
Marwick, A., & Lewis, R. (2017). Media manipulation and disinformation online. New York,
NY: Data & Society Research Institute.
McEwan, B. (2013). Sharing, caring, and surveilling on social network sties: An actor-partner
interdependence model investigation of Facebook relational maintenance.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, & Social Networking, 16, 863-869.
doi:10.1089/cyber.2012.0272
McEwan, B. (2015). Navigating new media networks: Understanding and managing
communication challenges in a networked society. Lanham, MD: Lexington Books.
McEwan, B., Fletcher, J. Eden, J., & Sumner, E. (2014). Development and validation of a
Facebook relational maintenance measure. Communication Methods & Measures, 8,
244-263. doi:10.1080/19312458.2014.967844
McKenna, K. Y. A., & Bargh, J. A. (1998). Coming out in the age of the Internet: Identity
‘demarginalization’ through virtual group participation. Journal of Personality & Social
Psychology, 75, 681-694. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.3.681
SOCIAL MEDIA 28
Messing, S., & Westwood, S. J. (2014). Selective exposure in the age of social media:
Endorsements trump partisan source affiliation when selecting news online.
Communication Research, 41, 1042-1063. doi:10.1177/0093650212466406
Mikal, J. P., Rice, R. E., Abeyta, A., & DeVilbiss, J. (2013). Transition, stress and computer-
mediated social support. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, A40-A53.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2012.12.012
O’Sullivan, P. B., & Carr, C. T. (2018). Masspersonal communication: A model bridging the
mass-interpersonal divide. New Media & Society, 20, 1161-1180.
doi:10.1177/1461444816686104
Papacharissi, Z. (2002). The virtual sphere: The internet as a public sphere. New Media &
Society, 4, 9-27. doi: 10.1177/14614440222226244
Parks, M. R., & Floyd, K. (1996). Making friends in cyberspace. Journal of Computer-
Mediated Communication, 1(4). doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.1996.tb00176.x
Pearce, K. E., & Kendzior, S. (2012). Networked authoritarianism and social media in
Azerbaijan. Journal of Communication, 62, 283-298. doi:10.1111/j.1460-
2466.2012.01633.x
Rains, S. A. (2018). Coping with illness digitally. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Rheingold, H. (1993). The virtual community: Homesteading on the electronic frontier.
Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Rice, R. E., Evans, S. K., Pearce, K. E., Sivunen, A., Vitak, J., & Treem, J. W. (2017).
Organizational media affordances: Operationalization and associations with media use.
Journal of Communication, 67, 106-130. doi:10.1111/jcom.12273
Rogers, E. M. (1962). Diffusion of innovations. New York, NY: Free Press.
SOCIAL MEDIA 29
Rosen, J. (2012). The right to be forgotten. Stanford Law Review Online, 64, 88-92. Retrieved
from https://www.stanfordlawreview.org/online/privacy-paradox-the-right-to-be-
forgotten/
Seabrook, E. M., Kern, M. L., & Rickard, N. S. (2016). Social networking sites, depression,
and anxiety: a systematic review. JMIR Mental Health, 3(4), e50. doi:
10.2196/mental.5842
Shu, K., Wang, S., Tang, J., Zafarani, R., & Liu, H. (2017). User identity linkage across online
social networks: A review. ACM SIGKDD Explorations Newsletter, 18(2), 5-17.
doi:10.1145/3068777.3068781
Smith, A., & Anderson, M. (2018). Social media use in 2018. Pew Research Center. Retrieved
from http://www.pewinternet.org/2018/03/01/social-media-use-in-2018/
Stoycheff, E., Liu, J., Wibowo, K. A., & Nanni, D. P. (2017). What have we learned about
social media by studying Facebook? A decade in review. New Media & Society, 19,
968-980. doi: 10.1177/1461444817695745
Stutzman, F., Gross, R., & Acquisti, A. (2013). Silent listeners: The evolution of privacy and
disclosure on Facebook. Journal of Privacy and Confidentiality, 4(2), 7-41.
doi:10.29012/jpc.v4i2.620
Sundar, S. S. (2007). Social psychology of interactivity in human-website interaction. In A. N.
Joinson, K. Y. A. McKenna, T. Postmes, & U.-D. Reips (Eds.), The Oxford handbook
of Internet psychology (pp. 89-104). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Taylor, T. L. (2018). Watch me play: Twitch and the rise of live streaming. Princeton, NJ:
Princeton University Press.
SOCIAL MEDIA 30
Tokunaga, R. S. (2010). Following you home from school: A critical review and synthesis of
research on cyberbullying victimization. Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 277-287.
doi: 10.1016/j.chb/2009/11.014
Treem, J., & Leonardi, P. (2013). Social media use in organizations: Exploring the affordances
of visibility, editability, persistence, and association. In C. T. Salmon (Ed.),
Communication yearbook (Vol. 38, pp. 0143-189). New York, NY: Routledge.
Tufecki, Z. (2017). Twitter and tear gas: The power and fragility of networked protest. New
Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Turkle, S. (1984). The second self: Computers and the human spirit. New York, NY: Simon &
Schuster.
Utz, S. (2000). Social information processing in MUDs: The development of friendships in
virtual worlds. Journal of Online Behavior, 1(1).
Vaidhyanathan, S. (2018). Anti-social media: How Facebook disconnects us and undermines
democracy. New York, NY: Oxford.
Valkenberg, P. M., Schouten, A. P., & Peter, J. (2005). Adolescents’ identity experiments on
the internet. New Media & Society, 7, 383-402. doi:10.1177/1461444805052282
Verduyn, P., Lee, D. S., Park, J., Shablack, H., Orvell, A., Bayer, J., ... & Kross, E. (2015).
Passive Facebook usage undermines affective well-being: Experimental and
longitudinal evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 480-488.
doi:10.1037/xge0000057
Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interpersonal, and
hyperpersonal interaction. Communication Research, 23, 3-43.
doi:10.1177/009365096023001001
SOCIAL MEDIA 31
Walther, J. B., & Jang, J. W. (2012). Communication processes in participatory websites.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 18, 2-15. doi:10.1111/j.1083-
6101.2012.01592.x
Walther, J. B., & Parks, M. R. (2002). Cues filtered out, cues filtered in: Computer-mediated
communication and relationships. In M. L. Knapp & J. A. Daly (Eds.), Handbook of
interpersonal communication (3rd ed., pp. 529-563). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Walther, J. B., Van Der Heide, B., Hamel, L. M., & Shulman, H. C. (2009). Self-generated
versus other-generated statements and impressions in computer-mediated
communication: A test of warranting theory using Facebook. Communication Research,
36, 229-253. doi:10.1177/0093650208330251
Wellman, B., & Hampton, K. (1999). Living networked on and offline. Contemporary
Sociology, 28, 648-654. doi: 10.2307/2655535
Wojcieszak, M. (2010). “Don’t talk to me”: Effects of ideologically homogenous online groups
and politically dissimilar offline ties on extremism. New Media & Society, 12, 637-655.
doi:10.1177/146144809342775
Wright, K. (2000). Computer-mediated social support, older adults, and coping. Journal of
Communication, 50, 100-118. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2000.tb02855.x
Yang, Q. (2017). Are social networking sites making health behavior change interventions
more effective? A meta-analytic review. Journal of Health Communication, 22, 223-
233. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2016.1271065
Yee, N. (2014). The Proteus paradox: How online games and virtual worlds change us--and how
they don’t. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
- Social Media
- Conceptualizing Social Media
- Universal Affordances of Social Media
- Other Social Media Affordances
- Types and Uses of Social Media
- Areas of Social Media Research and Theorizing
- Self-Presentation, Impression Management, and Identity Performance
- Disclosure and Privacy
- Relationship Development and Maintenance
- Information Diffusion and Evaluation
- Improving Social Media Research
- Emerging and Evolving Social Media Platforms

Get help from top-rated tutors in any subject.
Efficiently complete your homework and academic assignments by getting help from the experts at homeworkarchive.com